< Return to Video

Should you trust unanimous decisions? - Derek Abbott

  • 0:07 - 0:10
    Imagine a police line-up
    where ten witnesses
  • 0:10 - 0:16
    are asked to identify a bank robber
    they glimpsed fleeing the crime scene.
  • 0:16 - 0:18
    If six of them pick out the same person,
  • 0:18 - 0:21
    there's a good chance
    that's the real culprit,
  • 0:21 - 0:23
    and if all ten make the same choice,
  • 0:23 - 0:25
    you might think the case is rock solid,
  • 0:25 - 0:27
    but you'd be wrong.
  • 0:27 - 0:30
    For most of us,
    this sounds pretty strange.
  • 0:30 - 0:35
    After all, much of our society
    relies on majority vote and consensus,
  • 0:35 - 0:36
    whether its politics,
  • 0:36 - 0:37
    business,
  • 0:37 - 0:38
    or entertainment.
  • 0:38 - 0:42
    So it's natural to think
    that more consensus is a good thing.
  • 0:42 - 0:45
    And up until a certain point,
    it usually is.
  • 0:45 - 0:49
    But sometimes, the closer you start to get
    to total agreement,
  • 0:49 - 0:53
    the less reliable the result becomes.
  • 0:53 - 0:56
    This is called the paradox of unanimity.
  • 0:56 - 0:58
    The key to understanding
    this apparent paradox
  • 0:58 - 1:02
    is in considering the overall level
    of uncertainty
  • 1:02 - 1:06
    involved in the type of situation
    you're dealing with.
  • 1:06 - 1:10
    If we asked witnesses to identify
    the apple in this line up, for example,
  • 1:10 - 1:13
    we shouldn't be surprised
    by a unanimous verdict.
  • 1:13 - 1:18
    But in cases where we have
    reason to expect some natural variance,
  • 1:18 - 1:21
    we should also expect varied distribution.
  • 1:21 - 1:23
    If you toss a coin one hundred times,
  • 1:23 - 1:28
    you would expect to get heads
    somewhere around 50% of the time.
  • 1:28 - 1:32
    But if your results started
    to approach 100% heads,
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    you'd suspect that something was wrong,
  • 1:34 - 1:36
    not with your individual flips,
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    but with the coin itself.
  • 1:39 - 1:44
    Of course, suspect identifications aren't
    as random as coin tosses,
  • 1:44 - 1:48
    but they're not as clear cut
    as telling apples from bananas, either.
  • 1:48 - 1:54
    In fact, a 1994 study found
    that up to 48% of witnesses
  • 1:54 - 1:57
    tend to pick the wrong
    person out of a line-up,
  • 1:57 - 2:00
    even when many
    are confident in their choice.
  • 2:00 - 2:04
    Memory based on short glimpses
    can be unreliable,
  • 2:04 - 2:07
    and we often overestimate
    our own accuracy.
  • 2:07 - 2:08
    Knowing all this,
  • 2:08 - 2:12
    a unanimous identification starts to seem
    less like certain guilt,
  • 2:12 - 2:15
    and more like a systemic error,
  • 2:15 - 2:17
    or bias in the line-up.
  • 2:17 - 2:21
    And systemic errors don't just appear
    in matters of human judgement.
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    From 1993-2008,
  • 2:23 - 2:29
    the same female DNA was found
    in multiple crime scenes around Europe,
  • 2:29 - 2:34
    incriminating an elusive killer
    dubbed The Phantom of Heilbronn.
  • 2:34 - 2:40
    But the DNA evidence was so consistent
    precisely because it was wrong.
  • 2:40 - 2:44
    It turned out that the cotton swabs
    used to collect the DNA samples
  • 2:44 - 2:50
    had all been accidentally contaminated
    by a woman working in the swab factory.
  • 2:50 - 2:54
    In other cases, systematic errors arise
    through deliberate fraud,
  • 2:54 - 2:59
    like the presidential referendum held
    by Saddam Hussein in 2002,
  • 2:59 - 3:06
    which claimed a turnout of 100% of voters
    with all 100% supposedly voting in favor
  • 3:06 - 3:09
    of another seven-year term.
  • 3:09 - 3:11
    When you look at it this way,
  • 3:11 - 3:15
    the paradox of unanimity isn't actually
    all that paradoxical.
  • 3:15 - 3:18
    Unanimous agreement
    is still theoretically ideal,
  • 3:18 - 3:24
    especially in cases when you'd expect very
    low odds of variability and uncertainty,
  • 3:24 - 3:25
    but in practice,
  • 3:25 - 3:29
    achieving it in situations where
    perfect agreement is highly unlikely
  • 3:29 - 3:34
    should tell us that there's probably
    some hidden factor affecting the system.
  • 3:34 - 3:37
    Although we may strive for harmony
    and consensus,
  • 3:37 - 3:42
    in many situations, error and disagreement
    should be naturally expected.
  • 3:42 - 3:45
    And if a perfect result seems too good
    to be true,
  • 3:45 - 3:46
    it probably is.
Title:
Should you trust unanimous decisions? - Derek Abbott
Speaker:
Derek Abbott
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:03

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions