Return to Video

This is your brain on communication

  • 0:01 - 0:03
    Imagine that you invented a device
  • 0:03 - 0:05
    that can record my memories,
  • 0:05 - 0:07
    my dreams, my ideas,
  • 0:07 - 0:09
    and transmit it to your brain.
  • 0:09 - 0:12
    That would be a game-changing
    technology, right?
  • 0:12 - 0:15
    But in fact, we already
    possess this device,
  • 0:15 - 0:18
    and it's called human
    communication system
  • 0:18 - 0:20
    and effective storytelling.
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    And to understand
    how this device is working,
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    we have to look into our brains,
  • 0:25 - 0:29
    and we have to formulate the question
    in a slightly different manner.
  • 0:29 - 0:30
    Now we have to ask
  • 0:31 - 0:33
    how these neuron patterns in my brain
  • 0:33 - 0:36
    that are associated with
    my memories and ideas
  • 0:36 - 0:39
    are transmitted into your brains.
  • 0:39 - 0:43
    And we think there are two factors
    that enable us to communicate.
  • 0:43 - 0:46
    First, your brains now
    is physically coupled
  • 0:46 - 0:50
    to the soundwave that I'm
    transmitting to your brains,
  • 0:50 - 0:53
    and second, we developed
    a common neural protocol
  • 0:53 - 0:56
    that enabled us to communicate.
  • 0:56 - 0:57
    So how do we know that?
  • 0:57 - 0:59
    In my lab in Princeton,
  • 0:59 - 1:03
    we bring people to the FMRI scanner
    and we scan their brains
  • 1:03 - 1:07
    while they are either telling
    or listening to real life stories.
  • 1:07 - 1:09
    And to give you a sense
    of the stimulus we are using,
  • 1:09 - 1:11
    let me play you now 20 seconds
  • 1:11 - 1:13
    from a story that we used
  • 1:13 - 1:16
    told by a very talented storyteller,
  • 1:16 - 1:18
    Jim O'Grady.
  • 1:18 - 1:21
    (Audio) Jim O'Grady: So I'm
    banging out my story and I know it's good,
  • 1:21 - 1:24
    and then I start to make it better --
  • 1:24 - 1:26
    (Laughter) --
  • 1:26 - 1:29
    by adding an element of embellishment.
  • 1:29 - 1:33
    Reporters call this "making shit up."
  • 1:33 - 1:36
    (Laughter)
  • 1:36 - 1:40
    And they recommend against
    crossing that line.
  • 1:40 - 1:45
    But I had just seen the line crossed
    between a high-powered dean
  • 1:45 - 1:47
    and assault with a pastry,
    and I kinda liked it."
  • 1:47 - 1:50
    Uri Hasson: Okay, so let's look
    into your brains
  • 1:50 - 1:53
    and see what's happening when you're
    listening to these kinds of stories.
  • 1:53 - 1:55
    And that's not simple.
  • 1:55 - 1:58
    Let's start with one listener
    and one brain area:
  • 1:58 - 2:01
    the auditory cortex that processes
    the sounds that are coming from the ear.
  • 2:01 - 2:03
    And as you can see,
    in this particular brain area,
  • 2:03 - 2:07
    the responses are going up and down
    as the story is unfolding.
  • 2:07 - 2:10
    And now we can take these responses
    and compare it to the responses
  • 2:10 - 2:13
    in other listeners in the same brain area,
  • 2:13 - 2:16
    and we can ask how similar
    are the responses
  • 2:16 - 2:18
    across all these listeners.
  • 2:18 - 2:21
    So here you can see five listeners,
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    and we start to scan their brains
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    before the story is starting,
  • 2:25 - 2:28
    when they simply lying in the dark
    and waiting for the story to begin.
  • 2:28 - 2:32
    And as you can see, the brain area
    is going up and down in each one of them,
  • 2:32 - 2:34
    but the responses are very different
  • 2:34 - 2:36
    and not in sync.
  • 2:36 - 2:38
    However, immediately
    as the story is starting,
  • 2:38 - 2:41
    something amazing is happening.
  • 2:41 - 2:44
    (Audio) JO: So I'm banging out my story,
    and I know it's good,
  • 2:44 - 2:45
    and then I start to make it --
  • 2:45 - 2:49
    UH: Suddenly, you can see that
    the responses in all of the subjects
  • 2:49 - 2:50
    lock to the story
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    and now they are going up and down
  • 2:52 - 2:54
    in a very similar way
  • 2:54 - 2:55
    across all listeners.
  • 2:55 - 2:58
    And in fact, this is exactly
    what is happening now in your brains
  • 2:58 - 3:01
    when you listen to my sound speaking.
  • 3:01 - 3:04
    And we call this effect
    neural entrainment.
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    And to explain to you
    what is neural entrainment,
  • 3:07 - 3:10
    let me first explain
    what is physical entrainment.
  • 3:10 - 3:13
    So we'll look and see five metronomes,
  • 3:13 - 3:16
    and think of these five metronomes
    as five brains,
  • 3:16 - 3:18
    and similar to the listeners
    before the story starts,
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    these metronomes are going to click,
  • 3:21 - 3:23
    but they're going to click out of phase.
  • 3:23 - 3:27
    (Clicking)
  • 3:28 - 3:31
    Now see what will happen
    when I'm going to connect them together
  • 3:31 - 3:34
    by pressing them on
    these two cylinders.
  • 3:34 - 3:38
    (Clicking)
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    Now these two cylinders start to rotate,
  • 3:40 - 3:43
    and this rotation vibration
    is going through the wood
  • 3:43 - 3:46
    and going to couple
    all the metronomes together,
  • 3:46 - 3:48
    and now listen to the click.
  • 3:48 - 3:53
    (Clicking)
  • 3:58 - 4:01
    And this is what you call
    physical entrainment.
  • 4:01 - 4:05
    And now let's go back to the brain
    and ask, so what is driving
  • 4:05 - 4:06
    this neural entrainment?
  • 4:06 - 4:08
    Is it simply the sounds
    that the speaker is producing?
  • 4:08 - 4:09
    Or maybe it's the words.
  • 4:09 - 4:13
    Or maybe it's the meaning
    that the speaker is trying to convey.
  • 4:13 - 4:16
    So to test it, we did
    the following experiments.
  • 4:16 - 4:19
    First, we took the story
    and played it backwards.
  • 4:19 - 4:22
    And that had many
    of the auditory features,
  • 4:22 - 4:24
    but we moved the meaning.
  • 4:24 - 4:26
    And it sounds like that.
  • 4:26 - 4:28
    (Audio backwards)
  • 4:28 - 4:35
    And we flashed colors on the two brains
    to indicate brain areas that respond
  • 4:35 - 4:38
    very similarly across people.
  • 4:38 - 4:41
    And as you can see, this incoming sound
    induced entrainment or alignment
  • 4:41 - 4:44
    in all of the brains
    in auditory cortexes
  • 4:44 - 4:45
    that processes the sounds,
  • 4:45 - 4:48
    but it didn't spread
    deeper into the brain.
  • 4:48 - 4:51
    Now we can take these sounds
    and build words out of it.
  • 4:51 - 4:54
    So if we take Jim O'Grady
    and scramble the words,
  • 4:54 - 4:56
    we'll get a list of words.
  • 4:56 - 5:00
    (Word scramble)
  • 5:00 - 5:03
    And you can see that these words
    start to induce alignment
  • 5:03 - 5:06
    in early language areas,
    but not more than that.
  • 5:06 - 5:10
    And now we can take the word
    and start to build sentences out of it.
  • 5:10 - 5:16
    (Audio) JO: And they recommend
    against crossing that line.
  • 5:16 - 5:19
    He says, Dear Jim, good story,
  • 5:19 - 5:23
    nice details. Didn't she only
    know about him through me?
  • 5:23 - 5:26
    UH: And now you can see that
    the responses in all the language areas
  • 5:26 - 5:28
    that process the incoming language
    becoming aligned
  • 5:28 - 5:30
    and similar across all these centers.
  • 5:30 - 5:35
    However, only when we use the full
    engaging, coherent story
  • 5:35 - 5:37
    the responses spread deeper into the brain
  • 5:37 - 5:39
    into higher order areas
  • 5:39 - 5:42
    which include the frontal cortex
    and the parietal cortex,
  • 5:42 - 5:44
    and make all of them
    respond very similarly.
  • 5:44 - 5:48
    And we believe that these responses
    in higher order areas are induced
  • 5:48 - 5:50
    or become similar across listeners
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    because of the meaning
    conveyed by the speaker,
  • 5:53 - 5:55
    and not by words or sound.
  • 5:55 - 5:58
    And if we are right [??],
  • 5:58 - 6:00
    if I will tell you the exact same ideas
  • 6:00 - 6:03
    using two very different set of words,
  • 6:03 - 6:06
    your brain responses
    will still be similar.
  • 6:06 - 6:10
    And to test it, we did
    the following experiment in my lab.
  • 6:10 - 6:11
    We took the English story
  • 6:11 - 6:13
    and translated it to Russian.
  • 6:13 - 6:17
    And now you have two different sounds
    and linguistic systems
  • 6:17 - 6:20
    that convey the exact same meaning.
  • 6:20 - 6:23
    And you play the English story
    to the English listeners
  • 6:23 - 6:26
    and the Russian story
    to the Russian listeners
  • 6:26 - 6:29
    and now we can compare
    their responses across the groups.
  • 6:29 - 6:32
    And when we did that, we didn't
    see responses that are similar
  • 6:32 - 6:34
    in auditory cortexes,
  • 6:34 - 6:37
    in language areas, because the language
    and sound are very different.
  • 6:37 - 6:41
    However you can see that the responses
    in other areas were still similar
  • 6:41 - 6:43
    across these two groups.
  • 6:43 - 6:46
    And we believe that this is because
    they understood the story
  • 6:46 - 6:47
    in a very similar way
  • 6:47 - 6:52
    as we confirmed using a test
    after the story ended.
  • 6:52 - 6:55
    And we think that this alignment
    is necessary for communication.
  • 6:55 - 6:58
    For example, as you can tell,
  • 6:58 - 7:01
    I am not a native English speaker,
  • 7:01 - 7:03
    and I grew up with another language,
  • 7:03 - 7:05
    and the same might be
    for many of you in the audience,
  • 7:05 - 7:07
    and still we can communicate.
  • 7:07 - 7:12
    How come? We think we can communicate
    because we have this common [??]
  • 7:12 - 7:14
    to present meaning.
  • 7:14 - 7:18
    So so far, I've only talked about what's
    happening in the listener's brain,
  • 7:18 - 7:20
    in your brain when
    you're listening to talks.
  • 7:20 - 7:22
    But what's happening
    in the speaker's brain, in my brain,
  • 7:22 - 7:24
    when I'm speaking to you?
  • 7:24 - 7:26
    To look in the speaker's brain,
  • 7:26 - 7:29
    we asked the speaker
    to go into the scanner,
  • 7:29 - 7:31
    we scan his brains,
  • 7:31 - 7:35
    and then compare his brain responses
    to the brain responses of the listeners
  • 7:35 - 7:37
    listening to the story.
  • 7:37 - 7:41
    And you have to remember that
    producing speech and comprehending speech
  • 7:41 - 7:43
    are very different processes,
  • 7:43 - 7:46
    and we were asking, how similar are they?
  • 7:46 - 7:50
    And to our surprise, we saw
    that in these complex patterns
  • 7:50 - 7:55
    within the listener actually came
    from the speaker brain.
  • 7:55 - 7:59
    So production and comprehension
    involve very similar processes.
  • 7:59 - 8:02
    And we also found
    the stronger the similarity
  • 8:02 - 8:06
    between the listener's brain
    and the speaker's brain,
  • 8:06 - 8:08
    the better the communication.
  • 8:08 - 8:12
    So I know that if you
    are completely confused now,
  • 8:12 - 8:14
    and I do hope that this is not the case,
  • 8:14 - 8:16
    your brain responses
    is very different than mine.
  • 8:16 - 8:19
    But I also know that if
    you really understand me now,
  • 8:19 - 8:25
    then your brain and your brain
    and your brain are really similar to mine.
  • 8:25 - 8:29
    And now, let's take all this
    information together and ask,
  • 8:29 - 8:32
    how can we use it to transmit
    a memory that I have
  • 8:32 - 8:35
    from my brain to your brains?
  • 8:35 - 8:37
    Okay, so we did the following experiment.
  • 8:37 - 8:40
    We let people watch
    for the first time in their life
  • 8:40 - 8:44
    a TV episode from the BBC series
    "Sherlock" while we scanned their brains.
  • 8:44 - 8:47
    And then we asked them
    to go back to the scanner
  • 8:47 - 8:51
    and tell the story to another person
    that never watched the movie.
  • 8:51 - 8:53
    So let's be specific.
  • 8:53 - 8:55
    Think about this exact scene
  • 8:55 - 8:58
    when Sherlock is entering
    the cab in London
  • 8:58 - 9:00
    driven by the murderer he is looking for.
  • 9:00 - 9:03
    With me, as a viewer,
  • 9:03 - 9:07
    there is a specific brain patterns
    in my brain when I watch it.
  • 9:07 - 9:11
    Now, the exact same pattern,
    I can reactivate it in my brain again
  • 9:11 - 9:15
    by telling the world,
    Sherlock, London, murderer.
  • 9:15 - 9:19
    And now when I'm transmitting
    these words to your brains now,
  • 9:19 - 9:21
    you have to reconstruct it in your mind,
  • 9:21 - 9:24
    and in fact we see that
    this pattern emerging now
  • 9:24 - 9:26
    in your brains.
  • 9:26 - 9:28
    And we were really surprised to see
  • 9:28 - 9:30
    that the patterns that you have
    now in your brains
  • 9:30 - 9:32
    when I'm describing you these scenes
  • 9:32 - 9:35
    would be very similar
    to the pattern I had
  • 9:35 - 9:38
    when I watched this movie
    a few months ago in the scanner.
  • 9:38 - 9:40
    And this starts to tell you
    about the mechanism
  • 9:40 - 9:44
    by which we can tell stories
    and transmit information.
  • 9:44 - 9:45
    Because for example,
  • 9:45 - 9:49
    now you're listening really hard
    and try to understand what I'm saying,
  • 9:49 - 9:51
    and I know that it's not easy,
  • 9:51 - 9:55
    but I hope that in one point
    in the talk, we clicked and you got me.
  • 9:55 - 9:59
    And I think that in a few hours,
    few days, few months,
  • 9:59 - 10:01
    you're going to meet someone in a party,
  • 10:01 - 10:04
    And then you're going to tell him
    about these lectures,
  • 10:04 - 10:08
    and suddenly it will be as if
    he is standing now here with us.
  • 10:08 - 10:11
    And now you can see how we
    can take this mechanism
  • 10:11 - 10:13
    and try to transmit
  • 10:13 - 10:15
    memories and knowledge across people,
  • 10:15 - 10:17
    which is like wonderful, right?
  • 10:17 - 10:20
    But our ability to communicate relies
  • 10:20 - 10:23
    on our ability to have common ground.
  • 10:23 - 10:25
    Because for example,
  • 10:25 - 10:28
    if I'm going to use the British synonym
  • 10:28 - 10:31
    "hackney carriage" instead of "cab,"
  • 10:31 - 10:35
    I know that I'm going to be misaligned
    with most of you in the audience.
  • 10:35 - 10:37
    And this alignment depends
    not only on our ability to understand
  • 10:37 - 10:39
    the basic concept.
  • 10:39 - 10:43
    It also depends on our ability
    to develop common ground
  • 10:43 - 10:44
    and understanding
  • 10:44 - 10:46
    and shared belief systems.
  • 10:46 - 10:47
    Because we know that in many cases,
  • 10:47 - 10:52
    people understand the exact
    same story in very different ways.
  • 10:52 - 10:56
    So to test it in the lab,
    we did the following experiment.
  • 10:56 - 10:59
    We took a story by J.D. Salinger
  • 10:59 - 11:00
    in which a husband
  • 11:00 - 11:03
    lost track of his wife
    in the middle of a party
  • 11:03 - 11:06
    and he's calling his best friend asking,
    "Did you see my wife?"
  • 11:06 - 11:11
    For half of the subjects, we were telling
    that the wife was having
  • 11:11 - 11:13
    an affair with the best friend.
  • 11:13 - 11:18
    For the other hand, we were telling
    that the wife is loyal
  • 11:18 - 11:20
    and the husband is very jealous.
  • 11:20 - 11:23
    This one sentence before the story started
  • 11:23 - 11:26
    was enough to make the brain responses
    of all the people that believed
  • 11:26 - 11:28
    that the wife was having an affair
  • 11:28 - 11:31
    to be very similar
    in these high order areas
  • 11:31 - 11:33
    and different than the other group.
  • 11:33 - 11:36
    And if one sentence is enough
    to make your brain similar
  • 11:36 - 11:38
    to people that think like you
  • 11:38 - 11:41
    and very different than people
    that think differently than you,
  • 11:41 - 11:45
    think how this effect is going
    to be amplified in real life
  • 11:45 - 11:48
    when we are all listening
    to the exact same news item
  • 11:48 - 11:51
    after being exposed
    for days after days after days
  • 11:51 - 11:53
    to different media channels
  • 11:53 - 11:55
    like Fox News or New York Times
  • 11:55 - 11:59
    that give us very different
    perspectives on reality.
  • 11:59 - 12:02
    So let me summarize.
  • 12:02 - 12:04
    If everything worked as planned tonight,
  • 12:04 - 12:06
    I used my ability to vocalize sound
    to be coupled to your brains,
  • 12:06 - 12:11
    and I used this coupling
    to transmit my brain patterns
  • 12:11 - 12:15
    associated with my memories
    and ideas into your brains.
  • 12:15 - 12:21
    In this I start to reveal the hidden neural
    mechanism by which we communicate.
  • 12:21 - 12:24
    And we know that in the future
    it will enable us to improve
  • 12:24 - 12:26
    and facilitate communication.
  • 12:26 - 12:28
    But these studies also reveal
  • 12:28 - 12:32
    that communication relies
    on a common ground,
  • 12:32 - 12:34
    and we have to be really worried
    as a society,
  • 12:34 - 12:36
    if you're going to lose this common ground
  • 12:36 - 12:41
    then our ability to speak with people
    that are slightly different than us,
  • 12:41 - 12:44
    because we let a few very strong
    media channels
  • 12:44 - 12:46
    to take control over the mic
  • 12:46 - 12:49
    and manipulate and control
    the way we all think.
  • 12:49 - 12:52
    And I'm not sure how to fix it,
    because I'm only a scientist,
  • 12:52 - 12:55
    but maybe one way to do it
  • 12:55 - 12:57
    is to go back to the more
    natural way of communication,
  • 12:57 - 12:59
    which is a dialogue,
  • 12:59 - 13:02
    in which it's not only me
    speaking to you now,
  • 13:02 - 13:04
    but we have a more natural way of talking
  • 13:04 - 13:08
    in which I am speaking
    and I am listening,
  • 13:08 - 13:12
    and together we are trying to come
    to a common ground and new ideas.
  • 13:12 - 13:15
    Because after all,
    the people we are coupled to
  • 13:15 - 13:17
    define who we are,
  • 13:17 - 13:19
    and our desire to be coupled
    to another brain
  • 13:19 - 13:24
    is something very basic that's starting
    at a very early age.
  • 13:24 - 13:27
    So let me finish with an example
  • 13:27 - 13:29
    with my own private life
  • 13:29 - 13:33
    that I think is a good example
    of how coupling to other people
  • 13:33 - 13:36
    is really going to define who we are.
  • 13:36 - 13:40
    This my son Jonathan at a very early age.
  • 13:40 - 13:44
    And see how he developed
    a vocal game together with my wife
  • 13:44 - 13:49
    only from the desire and pure joy
    of being coupled to another human being.
  • 13:50 - 13:56
    (Vocal noise)
  • 14:03 - 14:06
    (Laughter)
  • 14:06 - 14:11
    Now, think how the ability of my son
    to be coupled to us
  • 14:11 - 14:15
    and other people in his life is going
    to shape the man he is going to become.
  • 14:15 - 14:17
    And think how you change on a daily basis
  • 14:17 - 14:22
    from the interaction and coupling
    to other people in your life.
  • 14:22 - 14:25
    So keep being coupled to other people.
  • 14:25 - 14:27
    Keep spreading your ideas,
  • 14:27 - 14:29
    because the sum of all of us together,
  • 14:29 - 14:32
    coupled, is greater than our parts.
  • 14:32 - 14:34
    Thank you.
  • 14:34 - 14:38
    (Applause)
Title:
This is your brain on communication
Speaker:
Uri Hasson
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
14:51
  • This transcript was updated on 8/17/16.

    At 9:11, the phrase "by telling the world" was changed to "by telling the word."

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions