Return to Video

Why we should trust scientists

  • 0:01 - 0:04
    Every day we face issues like climate change
  • 0:04 - 0:06
    or the safety of vaccines
  • 0:06 - 0:08
    where we have to answer questions whose answers
  • 0:08 - 0:12
    rely heavily on scientific information.
  • 0:12 - 0:15
    Scientists tell us that the world is warming.
  • 0:15 - 0:17
    Scientists tell us that vaccines are safe.
  • 0:17 - 0:19
    But how do we know if they are right?
  • 0:19 - 0:22
    Why should be believe the science?
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    The fact is, many of us actually
    don't believe the science.
  • 0:25 - 0:27
    Public opinion polls consistently show
  • 0:27 - 0:30
    that significant proportions of the American people
  • 0:30 - 0:33
    don't believe the climate is
    warming due to human activities,
  • 0:33 - 0:37
    don't think that there is
    evolution by natural selection,
  • 0:37 - 0:41
    and aren't persuaded by the safety of vaccines.
  • 0:41 - 0:44
    So why should we believe the science?
  • 0:44 - 0:48
    Well, scientists don't like talking about
    science as a matter of belief.
  • 0:48 - 0:50
    In fact, they would contract science with faith,
  • 0:50 - 0:53
    and they would say belief is the domain of faith.
  • 0:53 - 0:57
    And faith is a separate thing
    apart and distinct from science.
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    Indeed they would say religion is based on faith
  • 1:00 - 1:04
    or maybe the calculous of Pascal's wager.
  • 1:04 - 1:07
    Blaise Pascal was a 17th century mathematician
  • 1:07 - 1:10
    who tried to bring scientific reasoning to the question of
  • 1:10 - 1:12
    whether or not he should believe in God,
  • 1:12 - 1:14
    and his wager went like this:
  • 1:14 - 1:16
    well, if God doesn't exist
  • 1:16 - 1:19
    but I decide to believe in him
  • 1:19 - 1:21
    nothing much is really lost.
  • 1:21 - 1:22
    Maybe a few hours on Sunday.
  • 1:22 -
    [Laughter]
  • Not Synced
    But if he does exist and I don't believe in him,
  • Not Synced
    then I'm in deep trouble.
  • Not Synced
    And so Pascal said, we'd better believe in God.
  • Not Synced
    Or as one of my college professors said,
  • Not Synced
    "he clutched for the handmill of faith".
  • Not Synced
    He made that leap of faith
  • Not Synced
    leaving science and rationalism behind.
  • Not Synced
    Now the fact is though, for most of us
  • Not Synced
    most scientific claims are a leap of faith.
  • Not Synced
    We can't really judge scientific claims for ourselves in most cases.
  • Not Synced
    And indeed this is actually true for most scientists as well
  • Not Synced
    outside of their own specialties.
  • Not Synced
    So if you think about it, a geologist can't tell you
  • Not Synced
    wether a vaccine is safe.
  • Not Synced
    Most chemists are not experts in evolutionary theory.
  • Not Synced
    A physicist cannot tell you, despite the claims of some of them,
  • Not Synced
    wether or not tobacco causes cancer.
  • Not Synced
    So, if even scientists themselves have to make a leap of faith
  • Not Synced
    outside their own fields,
  • Not Synced
    then why do they accept the claims of other scientists?
  • Not Synced
    Why do they believe each other's claims?
  • Not Synced
    And should we believe those claims?
  • Not Synced
    So what I'd like to argue is yes, we should.
  • Not Synced
    But not for the reason that most of us think.
  • Not Synced
    Most of us were taught in school that the reason we should
  • Not Synced
    believe in science is because of the scientific method.
  • Not Synced
    We were taught that scientists follow a method
  • Not Synced
    and that this method guarantees the truth of their claims.
  • Not Synced
    The method that most of us were taught in school,
  • Not Synced
    we can call it the text book method,
  • Not Synced
    is the hypo-deductive method.
  • Not Synced
    According to the standard model, the textbook model,
  • Not Synced
    scientists develop hypotheses, they deduce the
  • Not Synced
    consequences for those hypotheses,
  • Not Synced
    and then they go out into the world and they say:
  • Not Synced
    Are those consequences true?
  • Not Synced
    Can we observe them taking place in the natural world?
  • Not Synced
    And if they are true, then the scientists say:
  • Not Synced
    Great, we know the hypothesis is correct.
  • Not Synced
    So there are many famous examples in the history
  • Not Synced
    of science of scientists doing exactly this.
  • Not Synced
    One of the most famous examples
  • Not Synced
    comes from the work of Albert Einstein.
  • Not Synced
    When Einstein developed the theory of general relativity
  • Not Synced
    one of the consequences of his theory
  • Not Synced
    was that space time wasn't just an empty void
  • Not Synced
    but that it actually had a fabric.
  • Not Synced
    And that that fabric was bent
  • Not Synced
    in the presence of massive objects like the sun.
  • Not Synced
    So if this theory were true then it meant that light
  • Not Synced
    as it passed the sun
  • Not Synced
    should actually be bent around it.
  • Not Synced
    That was a pretty startling prediction
  • Not Synced
    and it took a few years before scientists
  • Not Synced
    were able to test it.
  • Not Synced
    But they did test it in 1919
  • Not Synced
    and low and behold it turned out to be true.
  • Not Synced
    Starlight actually does bend as it travels around the sun.
  • Not Synced
    This was a huge confirmation of the theory.
  • Not Synced
    It was considered proof of the truth of this radical new idea
  • Not Synced
    and it was written up in many newspapers around the globe.
  • Not Synced
    Now sometimes this theory or this model
  • Not Synced
    is referred to as the deductive-nomological model.
  • Not Synced
    Meaning those academics like to make things complicated.
    [Laughter]
  • Not Synced
    But also because in the ideal case it's about laws.
  • Not Synced
    So nomological means having to do with laws.
  • Not Synced
    And in the ideal case, the hypothesis isn't just an idea,
  • Not Synced
    ideally it is a law of nature.
  • Not Synced
    Why does it matter that it is a law of nature?
  • Not Synced
    Because if it is a law, it can't be broken.
  • Not Synced
    If it's a law then it will always be true
  • Not Synced
    in all times and all places
  • Not Synced
    no matter what the circumstances are.
  • Not Synced
    And all of you know at least one example of a famous law.
  • Not Synced
    Einstein's famous equation, E=MC2,
  • Not Synced
    which tells us what the relationship is
  • Not Synced
    between energy and mass.
  • Not Synced
    And that relationship is true no matter what.
  • Not Synced
    It turns out though that there are
    several problems with this model.
  • Not Synced
    The main problem is that it's wrong.
  • Not Synced
    It's just not true. [Laughter]
  • Not Synced
    And I'm going to talk about three reasons why it's wrong.
  • Not Synced
    So the first reason is a logical reason,
  • Not Synced
    it's the problem of the fallacy of affirming the consequent.
  • Not Synced
    So that's another fancy academic way of saying
  • Not Synced
    that false theories can make true predictions.
  • Not Synced
    So just because the prediction comes true
  • Not Synced
    doesn't actually logically prove that the theory is correct.
  • Not Synced
    And I have a good example of that too,
    again from the history of science.
  • Not Synced
    This is a picture of the Ptolemaic universe
  • Not Synced
    with the Earth at the center of the universe
  • Not Synced
    and The Sun and the planets going around it.
  • Not Synced
    The Ptolemaic model was believed
  • Not Synced
    by many very smart people for many centuries.
  • Not Synced
    Well why?
  • Not Synced
    Well the answer is because it made
    lots of predictions that came true.
  • Not Synced
    The Ptolemaic system enabled astronomers
  • Not Synced
    to make accurate predictions of the motions of the planet.
  • Not Synced
    In fact more accurate predictions at first
  • Not Synced
    than the Copernican theory which we now would say is true.
  • Not Synced
    So that's one problem with the textbook model,
  • Not Synced
    a second problem is a practical problem
  • Not Synced
    and it's the problem of auxiliary hypotheses.
  • Not Synced
    Auxiliary hypotheses are assumptions
  • Not Synced
    that scientists are making,
  • Not Synced
    that they may or may not even be aware that they're making.
  • Not Synced
    So an important example of this comes from
  • Not Synced
    comes from the Copernican model
  • Not Synced
    which ultimately replaced the Ptolemaic system.
  • Not Synced
    So when Nicolaus Copernicus said,
  • Not Synced
    actually the Earth is not the center of the universe,
  • Not Synced
    the sun is the center of the solar system,
  • Not Synced
    the Earth moves around the sun.
  • Not Synced
    Scientists said, well okay, Nicolaus, if that's true
  • Not Synced
    we ought to be able to detect the motion
  • Not Synced
    of the Earth around the sun.
  • Not Synced
    And so this slide here illustrates a concept
  • Not Synced
    known as stellar parallax.
  • Not Synced
    And astronomers said, if the Earth is moving
  • Not Synced
    and we look at a prominent star, let's say, Sirius.
  • Not Synced
    Well I know I'm in Manhattan so you guys can't see the stars,
  • Not Synced
    but imagine you're out in the country,
    imagine you chose that rural life.
  • Not Synced
    And we look at a star in December, we see that star
  • Not Synced
    against the backdrop of distant stars.
  • Not Synced
    If we now make the same observation six months later
  • Not Synced
    when the Earth has moved to this position in June,
  • Not Synced
    we look at that same star and we see it against a different backdrop.
  • Not Synced
    That difference, that angular difference, is the stellar parallax.
  • Not Synced
    So this is the prediction that the Copernican model makes,
  • Not Synced
    astronomers looked for the stellar parallax
  • Not Synced
    and they found nothing, nothing at all.
  • Not Synced
    And many people argued that this proved
    that the Copernican model was false.
  • Not Synced
    So what happened?
  • Not Synced
    Well in hindsight we can say that astronomers were making
  • Not Synced
    two auxiliary hypotheses, both of which
  • Not Synced
    we would now say were incorrect.
  • Not Synced
    The first was an assumption about the size of the Earth's orbit.
  • Not Synced
    Astronomers were assuming that the Earth's orbit was large
  • Not Synced
    relative to the stars.
  • Not Synced
    Today we would draw the picture more like this,
  • Not Synced
    this comes from NASA,
  • Not Synced
    and you see the Earth's orbit is actually quite small.
  • Not Synced
    In fact, it's actually much smaller even than shown here.
  • Not Synced
    The stellar parallax therefore,
  • Not Synced
    is very small and actually very hard to detect.
  • Not Synced
    And that leads to the second reason
  • Not Synced
    why the prediction didn't work,
  • Not Synced
    because scientists were also assuming
  • Not Synced
    that the telescopes they had were sensitive enough
  • Not Synced
    to detect the parallax.
  • Not Synced
    And that turned out not to be true.
  • Not Synced
    It wasn't until the 19th century that scientists were able to detect
  • Not Synced
    the stellar parallax.
  • Not Synced
    So, there's a third problem as well.
  • Not Synced
    The third problem is simply a factual problem
  • Not Synced
    that a lot of science doesn't fit the textbook model.
  • Not Synced
    A lot of science isn't deductive at all, it's actually inductive.
  • Not Synced
    And by that we mean that scientists don't necessarily
  • Not Synced
    start with theories and hypotheses, often they just
  • Not Synced
    start with observations of stuff going on in the world.
  • Not Synced
    And the most famous example of that is one of the most
  • Not Synced
    famous scientists who ever lived, Charles Darwin.
  • Not Synced
    When Darwin went out as a young
    man on the voyage of the Beagle,
  • Not Synced
    he didn't have a hypothesis, he didn't have a theory.
  • Not Synced
    He just knew that he wanted to have a career as a scientist
  • Not Synced
    and he started to collect data.
  • Not Synced
    Mainly he knew that he hated medicine
  • Not Synced
    because the sight of blood made him sick so
  • Not Synced
    he had to have an alternative career path.
  • Not Synced
    So he started collecting data.
  • Not Synced
    And he collected many things including his famous finches.
  • Not Synced
    When he collected these finches he through them in a bag
  • Not Synced
    and he had no idea what they meant.
  • Not Synced
    Many years later back in London,
  • Not Synced
    Darwin looked at his data again and began to develop
  • Not Synced
    an explanation
  • Not Synced
    and that explanation was the theory of natural selection.
  • Not Synced
    Besides inductive science,
  • Not Synced
    scientists also often participate in modeling.
  • Not Synced
    One of the things scientists want to do in life
  • Not Synced
    is to explain the causes of things.
  • Not Synced
    And how do we do that?
  • Not Synced
    Well, one way you can do it is to build a model
  • Not Synced
    that tests an idea.
  • Not Synced
    So this is a picture of Henry Cadell,
  • Not Synced
    who was a Scottish geologist in the 19th century.
  • Not Synced
    You can tell he's Scottish because he's wearing
  • Not Synced
    a deerstalker cap and Wellington boots.
  • Not Synced
    (Laughter)
  • Not Synced
    And Cadell wanted to answer the question,
  • Not Synced
    how are mountains formed?
  • Not Synced
    And one of the things he had observed
  • Not Synced
    is that if you look at mountains
    like the Appalachians,
  • Not Synced
    you often find that the rocks in them
  • Not Synced
    are folded,
  • Not Synced
    and they're folded in a particular way,
  • Not Synced
    which suggested to him
  • Not Synced
    that they were actually being
    compressed from the side.
  • Not Synced
    And this idea would later play a major role
  • Not Synced
    in discussions of continental drift.
  • Not Synced
    So he built this model, this crazy contraption
  • Not Synced
    with levers and wood and here's his wheelbarrow,
  • Not Synced
    buckets, a big sledgehammer.
  • Not Synced
    I don't know why he's got the Wellington boots.
  • Not Synced
    Maybe it's going to rain.
  • Not Synced
    And he created this physical model in order
  • Not Synced
    to demonstrate that you could in fact create
  • Not Synced
    patterns in rocks, or at least in this case in mud,
  • Not Synced
    that looked a lot like mountains
  • Not Synced
    if you compressed them from the side.
  • Not Synced
    So it was an argument about
    the cause of mountains.
  • Not Synced
    Nowadays, most scientists prefer to work inside,
  • Not Synced
    so they don't build physical models so much
  • Not Synced
    as to make computer simulations.
  • Not Synced
    But a computer simulation is a kind of a model.
  • Not Synced
    It's a model that's made with mathematics,
  • Not Synced
    and like the physical models of the 19th century,
  • Not Synced
    it's very important for thinking about causes.
  • Not Synced
    So one of the big questions
    to do with climate change,
  • Not Synced
    we have tremendous amounts of evidence
  • Not Synced
    that the earth is warming up.
  • Not Synced
    This slide here, the black line shows
  • Not Synced
    the measurements that scientists have taken
  • Not Synced
    for the last 150 years
  • Not Synced
    showing that the earth's temperature
  • Not Synced
    has steadily increased,
  • Not Synced
    and you can see in particular
    that in the last 50 years
  • Not Synced
    there's been this dramatic increase
  • Not Synced
    of nearly one degree Centigrade,
  • Not Synced
    or almost two degrees Fahrenheit.
  • Not Synced
    So what, though, is driving that change?
  • Not Synced
    How can we know what's causing
  • Not Synced
    the observed warming?
  • Not Synced
    Well, scientists can model it
  • Not Synced
    using a computer simulation.
  • Not Synced
    So this diagram illustrates a computer simulation
  • Not Synced
    that has looked at all the different factors
  • Not Synced
    that we know can influence the earth's climate,
  • Not Synced
    so sulfate particles from air pollution,
  • Not Synced
    volcanic dust from volcanic eruptions,
  • Not Synced
    changes in solar radiation,
  • Not Synced
    and, of course, greenhouse gases.
  • Not Synced
    And they asked the question,
  • Not Synced
    what set of variables put into a model
  • Not Synced
    will reproduce what we actually see in real life?
  • Not Synced
    So here is the real life in black.
  • Not Synced
    Here's the model in this light grey,
  • Not Synced
    and the answer is
  • Not Synced
    a model that includes, it's the answer E on that SAT,
  • Not Synced
    all of the above.
  • Not Synced
    The only way you can reproduce
  • Not Synced
    the observed temperature measurements
  • Not Synced
    is with all of these things put together,
  • Not Synced
    including greenhouse gases,
  • Not Synced
    and in particular you can see that the increase
  • Not Synced
    in greenhouse gases tracks
  • Not Synced
    this very dramatic increase in temperature
  • Not Synced
    over the last 50 years.
  • Not Synced
    And so this is why climate scientists say
  • Not Synced
    it's not just that we know that
    climate change is happening,
  • Not Synced
    we know that greenhouse gases are a major part
  • Not Synced
    of the reason why.
  • Not Synced
    So now because there all these different things
  • Not Synced
    that scientists do,
  • Not Synced
    the philosopher Paul Feyerabend famously said,
  • Not Synced
    "The only principle in science
  • Not Synced
    that doesn't inhibit progress is: anything goes."
  • Not Synced
    Now this quotation has often
    been taken out of context,
  • Not Synced
    because Feyerabend was not actually saying
  • Not Synced
    that in science anything goes.
  • Not Synced
    What he was saying was,
  • Not Synced
    actually the full quotation is,
  • Not Synced
    "If you press me to say
  • Not Synced
    what is the method of science,
  • Not Synced
    I would have to say: anything goes."
  • Not Synced
    What he was trying to say
  • Not Synced
    is that scientists do a lot of different things.
  • Not Synced
    Scientists are creative.
  • Not Synced
    But then this pushes the question back:
  • Not Synced
    if scientists don't use a single method,
  • Not Synced
    then how do they decide
  • Not Synced
    what's right and what's wrong?
  • Not Synced
    And who judges?
  • Not Synced
    And the answer is, scientists judge,
  • Not Synced
    and they judge by judging evidence.
  • Not Synced
    Scientists collect evidence in many different ways,
  • Not Synced
    but however they collect it,
  • Not Synced
    they have to subject it to scrutiny.
  • Not Synced
    And this led to sociologist Robert Merton
  • Not Synced
    to focus on this question of how scientists
  • Not Synced
    scrutinize data and evidence,
  • Not Synced
    and he said they do it in a way he called
  • Not Synced
    "organized skepticism."
  • Not Synced
    And by that he meant it's organized
  • Not Synced
    because they do it collectively,
  • Not Synced
    they do it as a group,
  • Not Synced
    and skepticism, because they do it from a position
  • Not Synced
    of distrust.
  • Not Synced
    That is to say, the burden of proof
  • Not Synced
    is on the person with a novel claim.
  • Not Synced
    And in this sense, science
    is intrinsically conservative.
  • Not Synced
    It's quite hard to persuade the scientific community
  • Not Synced
    to say, "Yes, we know something, this is true."
  • Not Synced
    So despite the popularity of the concept
  • Not Synced
    of paradigm shifts,
  • Not Synced
    what we find is that actually,
  • Not Synced
    really major changes in scientific thinking
  • Not Synced
    are relatively rare in the history of science.
  • Not Synced
    So finally that brings us to one more idea:
  • Not Synced
    if scientists judge evidence collectively,
  • Not Synced
    this has led historians to focus on the question
  • Not Synced
    of consensus,
  • Not Synced
    and to say that at the end of the day,
  • Not Synced
    what science is,
  • Not Synced
    what scientific knowledge is,
  • Not Synced
    is the consensus of the scientific experts
  • Not Synced
    who through this process of organized scrutiny,
  • Not Synced
    collective scrutiny,
  • Not Synced
    have judged the evidence
  • Not Synced
    and come to a conclusion about it,
  • Not Synced
    either yea or nay.
  • Not Synced
    So we can think of scientific knowledge
  • Not Synced
    as a consensus of experts.
  • Not Synced
    We can also think of science as being
  • Not Synced
    a kind of a jury,
  • Not Synced
    except it's a very special kind of jury.
  • Not Synced
    It's not a jury of your peers,
  • Not Synced
    it's a jury of geeks.
  • Not Synced
    It's a jury of men and women with Ph.D's,
  • Not Synced
    and unlike a conventional jury,
  • Not Synced
    which has only two choices,
  • Not Synced
    guilty or not guilty,
  • Not Synced
    the scientific jury actually has a number of choices.
  • Not Synced
    Scientists can say yes, something's true.
  • Not Synced
    Scientists can say no, it's false.
  • Not Synced
    Or, they can say, well it might be true
  • Not Synced
    but we need to work more
    and collect more evidence.
  • Not Synced
    Or, they can say it might be true,
  • Not Synced
    but we don't know how to answer the question
  • Not Synced
    and we're going to put it aside
  • Not Synced
    and maybe come back to it later.
  • Not Synced
    That's what scientists call "intractable."
  • Not Synced
    But this leads us to one final problem:
  • Not Synced
    if science is what scientists say it is,
  • Not Synced
    then isn't that just an appeal to authority?
  • Not Synced
    And weren't we all taught in school
  • Not Synced
    that the appeal to authority is a logical fallacy?
  • Not Synced
    Well, here's the paradox of modern science,
  • Not Synced
    the paradox of the conclusions I think historians
  • Not Synced
    and philosophers and sociologists have come to,
  • Not Synced
    that actually science is the appeal to authority,
  • Not Synced
    but it's not the authority of the individual,
  • Not Synced
    no matter how smart that individual is,
  • Not Synced
    like Plato or Socrates or Einstein.
  • Not Synced
    It's the authority of the collective community.
  • Not Synced
    You can think of it is a kind of wisdom of the crowd,
  • Not Synced
    but a very special kind of crowd.
  • Not Synced
    Science does appeal to authority,
  • Not Synced
    but it's not based on any individual,
  • Not Synced
    no matter how smart that individual may be.
  • Not Synced
    It's based on the collective wisdom,
  • Not Synced
    the collective knowledge, the collective work,
  • Not Synced
    of all of the scientists who have worked
  • Not Synced
    on a particular problem.
  • Not Synced
    Scientists have a kind of culture of collective distrust,
  • Not Synced
    this "show me" culture,
  • Not Synced
    illustrated by this nice woman here
  • Not Synced
    showing her colleagues her evidence.
  • Not Synced
    Of course, these people don't
    really look like scientists,
  • Not Synced
    because they're much too happy.
  • Not Synced
    (Laughter)
  • Not Synced
    Okay, so that brings me to my final point.
  • Not Synced
    Most of us get up in the morning.
  • Not Synced
    Most of us trust our cars.
  • Not Synced
    Well, see, now I think, I'm in Manhattan,
  • Not Synced
    this is a bad analogy,
  • Not Synced
    but most Americans who don't live in Manhattan
  • Not Synced
    get up in the morning and get in their cars
  • Not Synced
    and turn on that ignition, and their cars work,
  • Not Synced
    and they work incredibly well.
  • Not Synced
    The modern automobile hardly ever breaks down.
  • Not Synced
    So why is that? Why do cars work so well?
  • Not Synced
    It's not because of the genius of Henry Ford
  • Not Synced
    or Carl Benz or even Elon Musk.
  • Not Synced
    It's because the modern automobile
  • Not Synced
    is the product of more than 100 years of work
  • Not Synced
    by hundreds and thousands
  • Not Synced
    and tens of thousands of people.
  • Not Synced
    The modern automobile is the product
  • Not Synced
    of the collected work and wisdom and experience
  • Not Synced
    of every man and woman who has ever worked
  • Not Synced
    on a car,
  • Not Synced
    and the reliability of the technology is the result
  • Not Synced
    of that accumulated effort.
  • Not Synced
    We benefit not just from the benefit of Benz
  • Not Synced
    and Ford and Musk
  • Not Synced
    but from the collective intelligence and hard work
  • Not Synced
    of all of the people who have worked
  • Not Synced
    on the modern car.
  • Not Synced
    And the same is true of science,
  • Not Synced
    only science is even older.
  • Not Synced
    Our basis for trust in science is actually the same
  • Not Synced
    as our basis in trust in technology,
  • Not Synced
    and the same as our basis for trust in anything,
  • Not Synced
    namely, experience.
  • Not Synced
    But it shouldn't be blind trust
  • Not Synced
    anymore than we would have blind trust in anything.
  • Not Synced
    Our trust in science, like science itself,
  • Not Synced
    should be based on evidence,
  • Not Synced
    and that means that scientists
  • Not Synced
    have to become better communicators.
  • Not Synced
    They have to explain to us not just what they know
  • Not Synced
    but how they know it,
  • Not Synced
    and it means that we have
    to become better listeners.
  • Not Synced
    Thank you very much.
  • Not Synced
    (Applause)
Title:
Why we should trust scientists
Speaker:
Naomi Oreskes
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
19:14
  • 2:50 is the hypothetical deductive method. --> the hypothetico-deductive method.

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions