Return to Video

What happens when you remove the hippocampus?

  • 0:07 - 0:10
    On September 1st, 1953,
  • 0:10 - 0:13
    William Scoville used a hand crank
    and a cheap drill saw
  • 0:13 - 0:18
    to bore into a young man's skull,
    cutting away vital pieces of his brain
  • 0:18 - 0:21
    and sucking them out through a metal tube.
  • 0:21 - 0:25
    But this wasn't a scene from a horror film
    or a gruesome police report.
  • 0:25 - 0:30
    Dr. Scoville was one of the most
    renowned neurosurgeons of his time,
  • 0:30 - 0:35
    and the young man was Henry Molaison,
    the famous patient known as "H.M.",
  • 0:35 - 0:40
    whose case provided amazing insights
    into how our brains work.
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    As a boy, Henry had cracked
    his skull in an accident
  • 0:44 - 0:50
    and soon began having seizures, blacking out
    and losing control of bodily functions.
  • 0:50 - 0:54
    After enduring years of frequent episodes,
    and even dropping out of high school,
  • 0:54 - 0:57
    the desperate young man
    had turned to Dr. Scoville,
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    a daredevil known for risky surgeries.
  • 1:00 - 1:04
    Partial lobotomies had been used
    for decades to treat mental patients
  • 1:04 - 1:08
    based on the notion that
    mental functions were strictly localized
  • 1:08 - 1:11
    to corresponding brain areas.
  • 1:11 - 1:14
    Having successfully used them
    to reduce seizures in psychotics,
  • 1:14 - 1:17
    Scoville decided to remove
    H.M.'s hippocampus,
  • 1:17 - 1:21
    a part of the limbic system
    that was associated with emotion
  • 1:21 - 1:24
    but whose function was unknown.
  • 1:24 - 1:26
    At first glance,
    the operation had succeeded.
  • 1:26 - 1:31
    H.M.'s seizures virtually disappeared,
    with no change in personality,
  • 1:31 - 1:33
    and his IQ even improved.
  • 1:33 - 1:37
    But there was one problem:
    His memory was shot.
  • 1:37 - 1:40
    Besides losing most of his memories
    from the previous decade,
  • 1:40 - 1:43
    H.M. was unable to form new ones,
    forgetting what day it was,
  • 1:43 - 1:48
    repeating comments,
    and even eating multiple meals in a row.
  • 1:48 - 1:52
    When Scoville informed another expert,
    Wilder Penfield, of the results,
  • 1:52 - 1:58
    he sent a Ph.D student named Brenda Milner
    to study H.M. at his parents' home,
  • 1:58 - 2:00
    where he now spent his days
    doing odd chores,
  • 2:00 - 2:05
    and watching classic movies
    for the first time, over and over.
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    What she discovered through
    a series of tests and interviews
  • 2:07 - 2:11
    didn't just contribute greatly
    to the study of memory.
  • 2:11 - 2:14
    It redefined what memory even meant.
  • 2:14 - 2:17
    One of Milner's findings shed light
    on the obvious fact
  • 2:17 - 2:22
    that although H.M. couldn't form new memories,
    he still retained information
  • 2:22 - 2:26
    long enough from moment to moment
    to finish a sentence or find the bathroom.
  • 2:26 - 2:28
    When Milner gave him a random number,
  • 2:28 - 2:31
    he managed to remember it
    for fifteen minutes
  • 2:31 - 2:33
    by repeating it to himself constantly.
  • 2:33 - 2:38
    But only five minutes later,
    he forgot the test had even taken place.
  • 2:38 - 2:42
    Neuroscientists had though of memory
    as monolithic,
  • 2:42 - 2:46
    all of it essentially the same
    and stored throughout the brain.
  • 2:46 - 2:50
    Milner's results were not only the first
    clue for the now familiar distinction
  • 2:50 - 2:53
    between short-term and long-term memory,
  • 2:53 - 2:56
    but show that each uses
    different brain regions.
  • 2:56 - 2:59
    We now know that memory formation
    involves several steps.
  • 2:59 - 3:05
    After immediate sensory data is temporarily
    transcribed by neurons in the cortex,
  • 3:05 - 3:07
    it travels to the hippocampus,
  • 3:07 - 3:12
    where special proteins work to strengthen
    the cortical synaptic connections.
  • 3:12 - 3:14
    If the experience was strong enough,
  • 3:14 - 3:17
    or we recall it periodically
    in the first few days,
  • 3:17 - 3:22
    the hippocampus then transfers the memory
    back to the cortex for permanent storage.
  • 3:22 - 3:25
    H.M.'s mind could form
    the initial impressions,
  • 3:25 - 3:29
    but without a hippocampus
    to perform this memory consolidation,
  • 3:29 - 3:33
    they eroded,
    like messages scrawled in sand.
  • 3:33 - 3:37
    But this was not the
    only memory distinction Milner found.
  • 3:37 - 3:41
    In a now famous experiment,
    she asked H.M. to trace a third star
  • 3:41 - 3:46
    in the narrow space between
    the outlines of two concentric ones
  • 3:46 - 3:49
    while he could only see
    his paper and pencil through a mirror.
  • 3:49 - 3:52
    Like anyone else performing such
    an awkward task for the first time,
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    he did horribly.
  • 3:54 - 3:58
    But surprisingly, he improved over
    repeated trials,
  • 3:58 - 4:01
    even though he had no memory
    of previous attempts.
  • 4:01 - 4:07
    His unconscious motor centers remembered
    what the conscious mind had forgotten.
  • 4:07 - 4:12
    What Milner had discovered was that the
    declarative memory of names, dates and facts
  • 4:12 - 4:17
    is different from the procedural memory
    of riding a bicycle or signing your name.
  • 4:17 - 4:20
    And we now know that procedural memory
  • 4:20 - 4:23
    relies more on the basal ganglia
    and cerebellum,
  • 4:23 - 4:26
    structures that were intact in H.M.'s brain.
  • 4:26 - 4:30
    This distinction between "knowing that"
    and "knowing how"
  • 4:30 - 4:33
    has underpinned all memory research since.
  • 4:33 - 4:38
    H.M. died at the age of 82 after
    a mostly peaceful life in a nursing home.
  • 4:38 - 4:43
    Over the years, he had been examined
    by more than 100 neuroscientists,
  • 4:43 - 4:46
    making his the most
    studied mind in history.
  • 4:46 - 4:49
    Upon his death, his brain was
    preserved and scanned
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    before being cut into over 2000
    individual slices
  • 4:52 - 4:58
    and photographed to form a digital map
    down to the level of individual neurons,
  • 4:58 - 5:02
    all in a live broadcast
    watched by 400,000 people.
  • 5:02 - 5:05
    Though H.M. spent most of his life
    forgetting things,
  • 5:05 - 5:08
    he and his contributions
    to our understanding of memory
  • 5:08 - 5:10
    will be remembered for
    generations to come.
Title:
What happens when you remove the hippocampus?
Speaker:
Sam Kean
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
05:26
  • Hi, I think there is a typo:
    2:38 though --> thought

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions