WEBVTT 00:00:06.688 --> 00:00:09.604 On September 1st, 1953, 00:00:09.604 --> 00:00:13.240 William Scoville used a hand crank and a cheap drill saw 00:00:13.240 --> 00:00:17.946 to bore into a young man's skull, cutting away vital pieces of his brain 00:00:17.946 --> 00:00:20.806 and sucking them out through a metal tube. 00:00:20.806 --> 00:00:25.111 But this wasn't a scene from a horror film or a gruesome police report. 00:00:25.111 --> 00:00:29.811 Dr. Scoville was one of the most renowned neurosurgeons of his time, 00:00:29.811 --> 00:00:35.114 and the young man was Henry Molaison, the famous patient known as "H.M.", 00:00:35.114 --> 00:00:40.466 whose case provided amazing insights into how our brains work. 00:00:40.466 --> 00:00:43.866 As a boy, Henry had cracked his skull in an accident 00:00:43.866 --> 00:00:49.610 and soon began having seizures, blacking out and losing control of bodily functions. 00:00:49.610 --> 00:00:53.901 After enduring years of frequent episodes, and even dropping out of high school, 00:00:53.901 --> 00:00:57.009 the desperate young man had turned to Dr. Scoville, 00:00:57.009 --> 00:01:00.202 a daredevil known for risky surgeries. 00:01:00.202 --> 00:01:04.259 Partial lobotomies had been used for decades to treat mental patients 00:01:04.259 --> 00:01:07.793 based on the notion that mental functions were strictly localized 00:01:07.793 --> 00:01:10.588 to corresponding brain areas. 00:01:10.588 --> 00:01:14.162 Having successfully used them to reduce seizures in psychotics, 00:01:14.162 --> 00:01:17.378 Scoville decided to remove H.M.'s hippocampus, 00:01:17.378 --> 00:01:21.142 a part of the limbic system that was associated with emotion 00:01:21.142 --> 00:01:23.781 but whose function was unknown. 00:01:23.781 --> 00:01:26.352 At first glance, the operation had succeeded. 00:01:26.352 --> 00:01:30.593 H.M.'s seizures virtually disappeared, with no change in personality, 00:01:30.593 --> 00:01:32.854 and his IQ even improved. 00:01:32.854 --> 00:01:36.616 But there was one problem: His memory was shot. 00:01:36.616 --> 00:01:39.907 Besides losing most of his memories from the previous decade, 00:01:39.907 --> 00:01:43.290 H.M. was unable to form new ones, forgetting what day it was, 00:01:43.290 --> 00:01:48.111 repeating comments, and even eating multiple meals in a row. 00:01:48.111 --> 00:01:52.333 When Scoville informed another expert, Wilder Penfield, of the results, 00:01:52.333 --> 00:01:57.918 he sent a Ph.D student named Brenda Milner to study H.M. at his parents' home, 00:01:57.918 --> 00:02:00.464 where he now spent his days doing odd chores, 00:02:00.464 --> 00:02:04.607 and watching classic movies for the first time, over and over. 00:02:04.607 --> 00:02:07.402 What she discovered through a series of tests and interviews 00:02:07.402 --> 00:02:10.608 didn't just contribute greatly to the study of memory. 00:02:10.608 --> 00:02:13.751 It redefined what memory even meant. 00:02:13.751 --> 00:02:16.692 One of Milner's findings shed light on the obvious fact 00:02:16.692 --> 00:02:21.577 that although H.M. couldn't form new memories, he still retained information 00:02:21.577 --> 00:02:26.185 long enough from moment to moment to finish a sentence or find the bathroom. 00:02:26.185 --> 00:02:28.358 When Milner gave him a random number, 00:02:28.358 --> 00:02:31.123 he managed to remember it for fifteen minutes 00:02:31.123 --> 00:02:33.474 by repeating it to himself constantly. 00:02:33.474 --> 00:02:38.064 But only five minutes later, he forgot the test had even taken place. 00:02:38.064 --> 00:02:41.812 Neuroscientists had though of memory as monolithic, 00:02:41.812 --> 00:02:45.800 all of it essentially the same and stored throughout the brain. 00:02:45.800 --> 00:02:49.868 Milner's results were not only the first clue for the now familiar distinction 00:02:49.868 --> 00:02:52.536 between short-term and long-term memory, 00:02:52.536 --> 00:02:56.125 but show that each uses different brain regions. 00:02:56.125 --> 00:02:59.369 We now know that memory formation involves several steps. 00:02:59.369 --> 00:03:04.834 After immediate sensory data is temporarily transcribed by neurons in the cortex, 00:03:04.834 --> 00:03:06.742 it travels to the hippocampus, 00:03:06.742 --> 00:03:12.066 where special proteins work to strengthen the cortical synaptic connections. 00:03:12.066 --> 00:03:13.749 If the experience was strong enough, 00:03:13.749 --> 00:03:16.682 or we recall it periodically in the first few days, 00:03:16.682 --> 00:03:22.147 the hippocampus then transfers the memory back to the cortex for permanent storage. 00:03:22.147 --> 00:03:25.371 H.M.'s mind could form the initial impressions, 00:03:25.371 --> 00:03:29.325 but without a hippocampus to perform this memory consolidation, 00:03:29.325 --> 00:03:33.361 they eroded, like messages scrawled in sand. 00:03:33.361 --> 00:03:36.565 But this was not the only memory distinction Milner found. 00:03:36.565 --> 00:03:41.194 In a now famous experiment, she asked H.M. to trace a third star 00:03:41.194 --> 00:03:45.753 in the narrow space between the outlines of two concentric ones 00:03:45.753 --> 00:03:49.373 while he could only see his paper and pencil through a mirror. 00:03:49.373 --> 00:03:52.490 Like anyone else performing such an awkward task for the first time, 00:03:52.490 --> 00:03:54.297 he did horribly. 00:03:54.297 --> 00:03:57.685 But surprisingly, he improved over repeated trials, 00:03:57.685 --> 00:04:01.151 even though he had no memory of previous attempts. 00:04:01.151 --> 00:04:06.644 His unconscious motor centers remembered what the conscious mind had forgotten. 00:04:06.644 --> 00:04:11.690 What Milner had discovered was that the declarative memory of names, dates and facts 00:04:11.690 --> 00:04:17.256 is different from the procedural memory of riding a bicycle or signing your name. 00:04:17.256 --> 00:04:19.500 And we now know that procedural memory 00:04:19.500 --> 00:04:22.852 relies more on the basal ganglia and cerebellum, 00:04:22.852 --> 00:04:26.117 structures that were intact in H.M.'s brain. 00:04:26.117 --> 00:04:29.556 This distinction between "knowing that" and "knowing how" 00:04:29.556 --> 00:04:32.960 has underpinned all memory research since. 00:04:32.960 --> 00:04:38.152 H.M. died at the age of 82 after a mostly peaceful life in a nursing home. 00:04:38.152 --> 00:04:42.556 Over the years, he had been examined by more than 100 neuroscientists, 00:04:42.556 --> 00:04:45.750 making his the most studied mind in history. 00:04:45.750 --> 00:04:48.915 Upon his death, his brain was preserved and scanned 00:04:48.915 --> 00:04:52.356 before being cut into over 2000 individual slices 00:04:52.356 --> 00:04:57.624 and photographed to form a digital map down to the level of individual neurons, 00:04:57.624 --> 00:05:02.017 all in a live broadcast watched by 400,000 people. 00:05:02.017 --> 00:05:04.684 Though H.M. spent most of his life forgetting things, 00:05:04.684 --> 00:05:07.602 he and his contributions to our understanding of memory 00:05:07.602 --> 00:05:10.013 will be remembered for generations to come.