Return to Video

A mouse. A laser beam. A manipulated memory.

  • 0:00 - 0:01
    Steve Ramirez: My first year of grad school,
  • 0:01 - 0:03
    I found myself in my bedroom
  • 0:03 - 0:06
    eating lots of Ben & Jerry's
  • 0:06 - 0:07
    watching some trashy TV
  • 0:07 - 0:11
    and maybe, maybe listening to Taylor Swift.
  • 0:11 - 0:12
    I had just gone through a breakup.
  • 0:12 - 0:14
    (Laughter)
  • 0:14 - 0:16
    So for the longest time, all I would do
  • 0:16 - 0:20
    is recall the memory of this person over and over again,
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    wishing that I could get rid of that gut-wrenching,
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    visceral "blah" feeling.
  • 0:25 - 0:27
    Now, as it turns out, I'm a neuroscientist,
  • 0:27 - 0:30
    so I knew that the memory of that person
  • 0:30 - 0:33
    and the awful, emotional undertones that color in that memory,
  • 0:33 - 0:35
    are largely mediated by separate brain systems.
  • 0:35 - 0:38
    And so I thought, what if we could go into the brain
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    and edit out that nauseating feeling
  • 0:40 - 0:43
    but while keeping the memory of that person intact?
  • 0:43 - 0:45
    Then I realized, maybe that's a little bit lofty for now.
  • 0:45 - 0:48
    So what if we could start off by going into the brain
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    and just finding a single memory to begin with?
  • 0:51 - 0:53
    Could we jump-start that memory back to life,
  • 0:53 - 0:57
    maybe even play with the contents of that memory?
  • 0:57 - 0:59
    All that said, there is one person in the entire world right now
  • 0:59 - 1:01
    that I really hope is not watching this talk.
  • 1:01 - 1:05
    (Laughter)
  • 1:05 - 1:08
    So there is a catch. There is a catch.
  • 1:08 - 1:11
    These ideas probably remind you of "Total Recall,"
  • 1:11 - 1:13
    "Eternal Sunshine of the Spotless Mind,"
  • 1:13 - 1:15
    or of "Inception."
  • 1:15 - 1:16
    But the movie stars that we work with
  • 1:16 - 1:18
    are the celebrities of the lab.
  • 1:18 - 1:20
    Xu Liu: Test mice.
  • 1:20 - 1:21
    (Laughter)
  • 1:21 - 1:24
    As neuroscientists, we work in the lab with mice
  • 1:24 - 1:28
    trying to understand how memory works.
  • 1:28 - 1:30
    And today, we hope to convince you that now
  • 1:30 - 1:33
    we are actually able to activate a memory in the brain
  • 1:33 - 1:36
    at the speed of light.
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    To do this, there's only two simple steps to follow.
  • 1:39 - 1:42
    First, you find and label a memory in the brain,
  • 1:42 - 1:46
    and then you activate it with a switch.
  • 1:46 - 1:47
    As simple as that.
  • 1:47 - 1:49
    (Laughter)
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    SR: Are you convinced?
  • 1:51 - 1:55
    So, turns out finding a memory in the brain isn't all that easy.
  • 1:55 - 1:57
    XL: Indeed. This is way more difficult than, let's say,
  • 1:57 - 2:00
    finding a needle in a haystack,
  • 2:00 - 2:02
    because at least, you know, the needle is still something
  • 2:02 - 2:05
    you can physically put your fingers on.
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    But memory is not.
  • 2:07 - 2:10
    And also, there's way more cells in your brain
  • 2:10 - 2:15
    than the number of straws in a typical haystack.
  • 2:15 - 2:18
    So yeah, this task does seem to be daunting.
  • 2:18 - 2:22
    But luckily, we got help from the brain itself.
  • 2:22 - 2:24
    It turned out that all we need to do is basically
  • 2:24 - 2:26
    to let the brain form a memory,
  • 2:26 - 2:30
    and then the brain will tell us which cells are involved
  • 2:30 - 2:32
    in that particular memory.
  • 2:32 - 2:34
    SR: So what was going on in my brain
  • 2:34 - 2:36
    while I was recalling the memory of an ex?
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    If you were to just completely ignore human ethics for a second
  • 2:39 - 2:40
    and slice up my brain right now,
  • 2:40 - 2:42
    you would see that there was an amazing number
  • 2:42 - 2:45
    of brain regions that were active while recalling that memory.
  • 2:45 - 2:48
    Now one brain region that would be robustly active
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    in particular is called the hippocampus,
  • 2:50 - 2:53
    which for decades has been implicated in processing
  • 2:53 - 2:55
    the kinds of memories that we hold near and dear,
  • 2:55 - 2:58
    which also makes it an ideal target to go into
  • 2:58 - 3:01
    and to try and find and maybe reactivate a memory.
  • 3:01 - 3:03
    XL: When you zoom in into the hippocampus,
  • 3:03 - 3:05
    of course you will see lots of cells,
  • 3:05 - 3:08
    but we are able to find which cells are involved
  • 3:08 - 3:10
    in a particular memory,
  • 3:10 - 3:12
    because whenever a cell is active,
  • 3:12 - 3:14
    like when it's forming a memory,
  • 3:14 - 3:18
    it will also leave a footprint that will later allow us to know
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    these cells are recently active.
  • 3:20 - 3:22
    SR: So the same way that building lights at night
  • 3:22 - 3:25
    let you know that somebody's probably working there at any given moment,
  • 3:25 - 3:29
    in a very real sense, there are biological sensors
  • 3:29 - 3:31
    within a cell that are turned on
  • 3:31 - 3:33
    only when that cell was just working.
  • 3:33 - 3:35
    They're sort of biological windows that light up
  • 3:35 - 3:37
    to let us know that that cell was just active.
  • 3:37 - 3:39
    XL: So we clicked part of this sensor,
  • 3:39 - 3:43
    and attached that to a switch to control the cells,
  • 3:43 - 3:47
    and we packed this switch into an engineered virus
  • 3:47 - 3:49
    and injected that into the brain of the mice.
  • 3:49 - 3:52
    So whenever a memory is being formed,
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    any active cells for that memory
  • 3:54 - 3:57
    will also have this switch installed.
  • 3:57 - 3:58
    SR: So here is what the hippocampus looks like
  • 3:58 - 4:01
    after forming a fear memory, for example.
  • 4:01 - 4:03
    The sea of blue that you see here
  • 4:03 - 4:05
    are densely packed brain cells,
  • 4:05 - 4:07
    but the green brain cells,
  • 4:07 - 4:09
    the green brain cells are the ones that are holding on
  • 4:09 - 4:11
    to a specific fear memory.
  • 4:11 - 4:13
    So you are looking at the crystallization
  • 4:13 - 4:15
    of the fleeting formation of fear.
  • 4:15 - 4:19
    You're actually looking at the cross-section of a memory right now.
  • 4:19 - 4:21
    XL: Now, for the switch we have been talking about,
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    ideally, the switch has to act really fast.
  • 4:24 - 4:27
    It shouldn't take minutes or hours to work.
  • 4:27 - 4:31
    It should act at the speed of the brain, in milliseconds.
  • 4:31 - 4:32
    SR: So what do you think, Xu?
  • 4:32 - 4:35
    Could we use, let's say, pharmacological drugs
  • 4:35 - 4:37
    to activate or inactivate brain cells?
  • 4:37 - 4:41
    XL: Nah. Drugs are pretty messy. They spread everywhere.
  • 4:41 - 4:44
    And also it takes them forever to act on cells.
  • 4:44 - 4:48
    So it will not allow us to control a memory in real time.
  • 4:48 - 4:52
    So Steve, how about let's zap the brain with electricity?
  • 4:52 - 4:54
    SR: So electricity is pretty fast,
  • 4:54 - 4:56
    but we probably wouldn't be able to target it
  • 4:56 - 4:58
    to just the specific cells that hold on to a memory,
  • 4:58 - 5:00
    and we'd probably fry the brain.
  • 5:00 - 5:03
    XL: Oh. That's true. So it looks like, hmm,
  • 5:03 - 5:06
    indeed we need to find a better way
  • 5:06 - 5:09
    to impact the brain at the speed of light.
  • 5:09 - 5:14
    SR: So it just so happens that light travels at the speed of light.
  • 5:14 - 5:18
    So maybe we could activate or inactive memories
  • 5:18 - 5:19
    by just using light --
  • 5:19 - 5:21
    XL: That's pretty fast.
  • 5:21 - 5:23
    SR: -- and because normally brain cells
  • 5:23 - 5:24
    don't respond to pulses of light,
  • 5:24 - 5:26
    so those that would respond to pulses of light
  • 5:26 - 5:29
    are those that contain a light-sensitive switch.
  • 5:29 - 5:31
    Now to do that, first we need to trick brain cells
  • 5:31 - 5:32
    to respond to to laser beams.
  • 5:32 - 5:33
    XL: Yep. You heard it right.
  • 5:33 - 5:35
    We are trying to shoot lasers into the brain.
  • 5:35 - 5:37
    (Laughter)
  • 5:37 - 5:40
    SR: And the technique that lets us do that is optogenetics.
  • 5:40 - 5:44
    Optogenetics gave us this light switch that we can use
  • 5:44 - 5:45
    to turn brain cells on or off,
  • 5:45 - 5:48
    and the name of that switch is channelrhodopsin,
  • 5:48 - 5:50
    seen here as these green dots attached to this brain cell.
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    You can think of channelrhodopsin as a sort of light-sensitive switch
  • 5:53 - 5:56
    that can be artificially installed in brain cells
  • 5:56 - 5:58
    so that now we can use that switch
  • 5:58 - 6:01
    to activate or inactivate the brain cell simply by clicking it,
  • 6:01 - 6:03
    and in this case we click it on with pulses of light.
  • 6:03 - 6:07
    XL: So we attach this light-sensitive switch of channelrhodopsin
  • 6:07 - 6:09
    to the sensor we've been talking about
  • 6:09 - 6:12
    and inject this into the brain.
  • 6:12 - 6:15
    So whenever a memory is being formed,
  • 6:15 - 6:17
    any active cell for that particular memory
  • 6:17 - 6:21
    will also have this light-sensitive switch installed in it
  • 6:21 - 6:23
    so that we can control these cells
  • 6:23 - 6:27
    by the flipping of a laser just like this one you see.
  • 6:27 - 6:30
    SR: So let's put all of this to the test now.
  • 6:30 - 6:32
    What we can do is we can take our mice
  • 6:32 - 6:35
    and then we can put them in a box that looks exactly like this box here,
  • 6:35 - 6:38
    and then we can give them a very mild footshock
  • 6:38 - 6:40
    so that they form a fear memory of this box.
  • 6:40 - 6:42
    They learn that something bad happened here.
  • 6:42 - 6:44
    Now with our system, the cells that are active
  • 6:44 - 6:47
    in the hippocampus in the making of this memory,
  • 6:47 - 6:50
    only those cells will now contain channelrhodopsin.
  • 6:50 - 6:53
    XL: When you are as small as a mouse,
  • 6:53 - 6:56
    it feels as if the whole world is trying to get you.
  • 6:56 - 6:58
    So your best response of defense
  • 6:58 - 7:01
    is trying to be undetected.
  • 7:01 - 7:03
    Whenever a mouse is in fear,
  • 7:03 - 7:04
    it will show this very typical behavior
  • 7:04 - 7:06
    by staying at one corner of the box,
  • 7:06 - 7:09
    trying to not move any part of its body,
  • 7:09 - 7:12
    and this posture is called freezing.
  • 7:12 - 7:17
    So if a mouse remembers that something bad happened in this box,
  • 7:17 - 7:19
    and when we put them back into the same box,
  • 7:19 - 7:21
    it will basically show freezing
  • 7:21 - 7:23
    because it doesn't want to be detected
  • 7:23 - 7:26
    by any potential threats in this box.
  • 7:26 - 7:28
    SR: So you can think of freezing as,
  • 7:28 - 7:30
    you're walking down the street minding your own business,
  • 7:30 - 7:31
    and then out of nowhere you almost run into
  • 7:31 - 7:34
    an ex-girlfriend or ex-boyfriend,
  • 7:34 - 7:36
    and now those terrifying two seconds
  • 7:36 - 7:38
    where you start thinking, "What do I do? Do I say hi?
  • 7:38 - 7:39
    Do I shake their hand? Do I turn around and run away?
  • 7:39 - 7:41
    Do I sit here and pretend like I don't exist?"
  • 7:41 - 7:44
    Those kind of fleeting thoughts that physically incapacitate you,
  • 7:44 - 7:47
    that temporarily give you that deer-in-headlights look.
  • 7:47 - 7:50
    XL: However, if you put the mouse in a completely different
  • 7:50 - 7:53
    new box, like the next one,
  • 7:53 - 7:56
    it will not be afraid of this box
  • 7:56 - 8:00
    because there's no reason that it will be afraid of this new environment.
  • 8:00 - 8:03
    But what if we put the mouse in this new box
  • 8:03 - 8:07
    but at the same time, we activate the fear memory
  • 8:07 - 8:10
    using lasers just like we did before?
  • 8:10 - 8:13
    Are we going to bring back the fear memory
  • 8:13 - 8:17
    for the first box into this completely new environment?
  • 8:17 - 8:19
    SR: All right, and here's the million dollar experiment.
  • 8:19 - 8:22
    Now to bring back to life the memory of that day,
  • 8:22 - 8:24
    I remember that the Red Sox had just won,
  • 8:24 - 8:26
    it was a green spring day,
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    perfect for going up and down the river
  • 8:28 - 8:31
    and then maybe going to the North End
  • 8:31 - 8:33
    to get some cannolis, #justsaying.
  • 8:33 - 8:36
    Now Xu and I, on the other hand,
  • 8:36 - 8:39
    were in a completely windowless black room
  • 8:39 - 8:42
    not making any ocular movement that even remotely resembles an eye blink
  • 8:42 - 8:45
    because our eyes were fixed onto a computer screen.
  • 8:45 - 8:47
    We were looking at this mouse here trying to activate a memory
  • 8:47 - 8:49
    for the first time using our technique.
  • 8:49 - 8:52
    XL: And this is what we saw.
  • 8:52 - 8:54
    When we first put the mouse into this box,
  • 8:54 - 8:57
    it's exploring, sniffing around, walking around,
  • 8:57 - 8:59
    minding its own business,
  • 8:59 - 9:01
    because actually by nature,
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    mice are pretty curious animals.
  • 9:03 - 9:05
    They want to know, what's going on in this new box?
  • 9:05 - 9:07
    It's interesting.
  • 9:07 - 9:10
    But the moment we turned on the laser, like you see now,
  • 9:10 - 9:13
    all of a sudden the mouse entered this freezing mode.
  • 9:13 - 9:18
    It stayed here and tried not to move any part of its body.
  • 9:18 - 9:19
    Clearly it's freezing.
  • 9:19 - 9:22
    So indeed, it looks like we are able to bring back
  • 9:22 - 9:24
    the fear memory for the first box
  • 9:24 - 9:27
    in this completely new environment.
  • 9:27 - 9:29
    While watching this, Steve and I
  • 9:29 - 9:32
    are as shocked as the mouse itself.
  • 9:32 - 9:33
    (Laughter)
  • 9:33 - 9:36
    So after the experiment, the two of us just left the room
  • 9:36 - 9:38
    without saying anything.
  • 9:38 - 9:41
    After a kind of long, awkward period of time,
  • 9:41 - 9:43
    Steve broke the silence.
  • 9:43 - 9:46
    SR: "Did that just work?"
  • 9:46 - 9:49
    XL: "Yes," I said. "Indeed it worked!"
  • 9:49 - 9:51
    We're really excited about this.
  • 9:51 - 9:53
    And then we published our findings
  • 9:53 - 9:55
    in the Journal of Nature.
  • 9:55 - 9:58
    Ever since the publication of our work,
  • 9:58 - 10:00
    we've been receiving numerous comments
  • 10:00 - 10:02
    from all over the Internet.
  • 10:02 - 10:06
    Maybe we can take a look at some of those.
  • 10:06 - 10:08
    ["OMGGGGG FINALLY... so much more to come, virtual reality, neural manipulation, visual dream emulation... neural coding, 'writing and re-writing of memories', mental illnesses. Ahhh the future is awesome"]
  • 10:08 - 10:10
    SR: So the first thing that you'll notice is that people
  • 10:10 - 10:13
    have really strong opinions about this kind of work.
  • 10:13 - 10:16
    Now I happen to completely agree with the optimism
  • 10:16 - 10:17
    of this first quote,
  • 10:17 - 10:19
    because on a scale of zero to Morgan Freeman's voice,
  • 10:19 - 10:22
    it happens to be one of the most evocative accolades
  • 10:22 - 10:23
    that I've heard come our way.
  • 10:23 - 10:25
    (Laughter)
  • 10:25 - 10:28
    But as you'll see, it's not the only opinion that's out there.
  • 10:28 - 10:29
    ["This scares the hell out of me... What if they could do that easily in humans in a couple of years?! OH MY GOD WE'RE DOOMED"]
  • 10:29 - 10:31
    XL: Indeed, if we take a look at the second one,
  • 10:31 - 10:33
    I think we can all agree that it's, meh,
  • 10:33 - 10:35
    probably not as positive.
  • 10:35 - 10:37
    But this also reminds us that,
  • 10:37 - 10:40
    although we are still working with mice,
  • 10:40 - 10:43
    it's probably a good idea to start thinking and discussing
  • 10:43 - 10:46
    about the possible ethical ramifications
  • 10:46 - 10:48
    of memory control.
  • 10:48 - 10:50
    SR: Now, in the spirit of the third quote,
  • 10:50 - 10:52
    we want to tell you about a recent project that we've been
  • 10:52 - 10:55
    working on in lab that we've called Project Inception.
  • 10:55 - 10:58
    ["They should make a movie about this. Where they plant ideas into peoples minds, so they can control them for their own personal gain. We'll call it: Inception."]
  • 10:58 - 11:02
    So we reasoned that now that we can reactivate a memory,
  • 11:02 - 11:05
    what if we do so but then begin to tinker with that memory?
  • 11:05 - 11:08
    Could we possibly even turn it into a false memory?
  • 11:08 - 11:12
    XL: So all memory is sophisticated and dynamic,
  • 11:12 - 11:15
    but if just for simplicity, let's imagine memory
  • 11:15 - 11:16
    as a movie clip.
  • 11:16 - 11:19
    So far what we've told you is basically we can control
  • 11:19 - 11:21
    this "play" button of the clip
  • 11:21 - 11:25
    so that we can play this video clip any time, anywhere.
  • 11:25 - 11:28
    But is there a possibility that we can actually get
  • 11:28 - 11:31
    inside the brain and edit this movie clip
  • 11:31 - 11:34
    so that we can make it different from the original?
  • 11:34 - 11:36
    Yes we can.
  • 11:36 - 11:38
    Turned out that all we need to do is basically
  • 11:38 - 11:42
    reactivate a memory using lasers just like we did before,
  • 11:42 - 11:46
    but at the same time, if we present new information
  • 11:46 - 11:50
    and allow this new information to incorporate into this old memory,
  • 11:50 - 11:52
    this will change the memory.
  • 11:52 - 11:56
    It's sort of like making a remix tape.
  • 11:56 - 11:59
    SR: So how do we do this?
  • 11:59 - 12:01
    Rather than finding a fear memory in the brain,
  • 12:01 - 12:02
    we can start by taking our animals,
  • 12:02 - 12:05
    and let's say we put them in a blue box like this blue box here
  • 12:05 - 12:08
    and we find the brain cells that represent that blue box
  • 12:08 - 12:10
    and we trick them to respond to pulses of light
  • 12:10 - 12:12
    exactly like we had said before.
  • 12:12 - 12:14
    Now the next day, we can take our animals and place them
  • 12:14 - 12:17
    in a red box that they've never experienced before.
  • 12:17 - 12:19
    We can shoot light into the brain to reactivate
  • 12:19 - 12:21
    the memory of the blue box.
  • 12:21 - 12:23
    So what would happen here if, while the animal
  • 12:23 - 12:24
    is recalling the memory of the blue box,
  • 12:24 - 12:27
    we gave it a couple of mild foot shocks?
  • 12:27 - 12:30
    So here we're trying to artificially make an association
  • 12:30 - 12:32
    between the memory of the blue box
  • 12:32 - 12:33
    and the foot shocks themselves.
  • 12:33 - 12:35
    We're just trying to connect the two.
  • 12:35 - 12:37
    So to test if we had done so,
  • 12:37 - 12:38
    we can take our animals once again
  • 12:38 - 12:40
    and place them back in the blue box.
  • 12:40 - 12:43
    Again, we had just reactivated the memory of the blue box
  • 12:43 - 12:45
    while the animal got a couple of mild foot shocks,
  • 12:45 - 12:47
    and now the animal suddenly freezes.
  • 12:47 - 12:50
    It's as though it's recalling being mildly shocked in this environment
  • 12:50 - 12:53
    even though that never actually happened.
  • 12:53 - 12:55
    So it formed a false memory,
  • 12:55 - 12:57
    because it's falsely fearing an environment
  • 12:57 - 12:58
    where, technically speaking,
  • 12:58 - 13:01
    nothing bad actually happened to it.
  • 13:01 - 13:03
    XL: So, so far we are only talking about
  • 13:03 - 13:06
    this light-controlled "on" switch.
  • 13:06 - 13:09
    In fact, we also have a light-controlled "off" switch,
  • 13:09 - 13:11
    and it's very easy to imagine that
  • 13:11 - 13:13
    by installing this light-controlled "off" switch,
  • 13:13 - 13:19
    we can also turn off a memory, any time, anywhere.
  • 13:19 - 13:21
    So everything we've been talking about today
  • 13:21 - 13:26
    is based on this philosophically charged principle of neuroscience
  • 13:26 - 13:30
    that the mind, with its seemingly mysterious properties,
  • 13:30 - 13:34
    is actually made of physical stuff that we can tinker with.
  • 13:34 - 13:35
    SR: And for me personally,
  • 13:35 - 13:37
    I see a world where we can reactivate
  • 13:37 - 13:39
    any kind of memory that we'd like.
  • 13:39 - 13:42
    I also see a world where we can erase unwanted memories.
  • 13:42 - 13:44
    Now, I even see a world where editing memories
  • 13:44 - 13:46
    is something of a reality,
  • 13:46 - 13:47
    because we're living in a time where it's possible
  • 13:47 - 13:50
    to pluck questions from the tree of science fiction
  • 13:50 - 13:52
    and to ground them in experimental reality.
  • 13:52 - 13:54
    XL: Nowadays, people in the lab
  • 13:54 - 13:56
    and people in other groups all over the world
  • 13:56 - 14:00
    are using similar methods to activate or edit memories,
  • 14:00 - 14:04
    whether that's old or new, positive or negative,
  • 14:04 - 14:07
    all sorts of memories so that we can understand
  • 14:07 - 14:08
    how memory works.
  • 14:08 - 14:10
    SR: For example, one group in our lab
  • 14:10 - 14:13
    was able to find the brain cells that make up a fear memory
  • 14:13 - 14:16
    and converted them into a pleasurable memory, just like that.
  • 14:16 - 14:19
    That's exactly what I mean about editing these kinds of processes.
  • 14:19 - 14:21
    Now one dude in lab was even able to reactivate
  • 14:21 - 14:23
    memories of female mice in male mice,
  • 14:23 - 14:26
    which rumor has it is a pleasurable experience.
  • 14:26 - 14:30
    XL: Indeed, we are living in a very exciting moment
  • 14:30 - 14:34
    where science doesn't have any arbitrary speed limits
  • 14:34 - 14:37
    but is only bound by our own imagination.
  • 14:37 - 14:39
    SR: And finally, what do we make of all this?
  • 14:39 - 14:41
    How do we push this technology forward?
  • 14:41 - 14:43
    These are the questions that should not remain
  • 14:43 - 14:45
    just inside the lab,
  • 14:45 - 14:47
    and so one goal of today's talk was to bring everybody
  • 14:47 - 14:50
    up to speed with the kind of stuff that's possible
  • 14:50 - 14:51
    in modern neuroscience,
  • 14:51 - 14:53
    but now, just as importantly,
  • 14:53 - 14:56
    to actively engage everybody in this conversation.
  • 14:56 - 14:58
    So let's think together as a team about what this all means
  • 14:58 - 15:01
    and where we can and should go from here,
  • 15:01 - 15:03
    because Xu and I think we all have
  • 15:03 - 15:05
    some really big decisions ahead of us.
  • 15:05 - 15:06
    Thank you.
    XL: Thank you.
  • 15:06 - 15:08
    (Applause)
Title:
A mouse. A laser beam. A manipulated memory.
Speaker:
Steve Ramirez and Xu Liu
Description:

Can we edit the content of our memories? It’s a sci-fi-tinged question that Steve Ramirez and Xu Liu are asking in their lab at MIT. Essentially, the pair shoot a laser beam into the brain of a living mouse to activate and manipulate its memory. In this unexpectedly amusing talk they share not only how, but -- more importantly -- why they do this. (Filmed at TEDxBoston.)

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
15:25

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions