Return to Video

The top 10 myths of psychology | Ben Ambridge | TEDxYouth@Manchester

  • 0:11 - 0:12
    So you've heard of your IQ,
  • 0:12 - 0:14
    your general intelligence,
  • 0:14 - 0:15
    but what's your Psy-Q?
  • 0:15 - 0:18
    How much do you knowabout what makes you tick,
  • 0:18 - 0:20
    and how good are youat predicting other peoples' behavior
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    or even your own?
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    And how much about what you thinkyou know about psychology is wrong?
  • 0:25 - 0:29
    So let's find out by counting downthe top 10 myths of psychology.
  • 0:29 - 0:32
    So you've probably heard it saidthat when it comes to their psychology,
  • 0:32 - 0:35
    it's almost as if men are from Marsand women are from Venus.
  • 0:35 - 0:37
    But how differentare men and women really?
  • 0:37 - 0:41
    So to find out, let's start by lookingat something on which men and women
  • 0:41 - 0:42
    really do differ
  • 0:42 - 0:45
    and plotting some psychologicalgender differences on the same scale.
  • 0:45 - 0:46
    So one thing that men and women
  • 0:46 - 0:49
    do really differ on is how farthey can throw a ball.
  • 0:49 - 0:51
    So if we look at the data for men here,
  • 0:51 - 0:53
    we see what is calleda normal distribution curve.
  • 0:53 - 0:55
    A few men can throw a ball really far,
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    and a few men not far at all,
  • 0:57 - 0:58
    but most a kind of average distance.
  • 0:58 - 1:00
    And women sharethe same distribution as well,
  • 1:00 - 1:03
    but actually there'squite a big difference.
  • 1:03 - 1:05
    In fact, the average mancan throw a ball further
  • 1:05 - 1:07
    than about 98 percent of all women.
  • 1:07 - 1:10
    So now let's look at whatsome psychological gender differences
  • 1:10 - 1:13
    look like on the same standardized scale.
  • 1:13 - 1:15
    So any psychologist will tell youthat men are better
  • 1:15 - 1:17
    at spacial awareness than women,
  • 1:17 - 1:19
    so things like map-reading, for example,
  • 1:19 - 1:20
    and it's true,
  • 1:20 - 1:22
    but let's have a lookat the size of this difference.
  • 1:22 - 1:25
    It's tiny: the lines are so close togetherthat they almost overlap.
  • 1:25 - 1:29
    In fact, the average woman is betterthan 33 percent of all men,
  • 1:29 - 1:31
    and of course, if that was 50 percent,
  • 1:31 - 1:33
    then the two genderswould be exactly equal.
  • 1:33 - 1:36
    And it's worth bearing in mindthat this difference
  • 1:36 - 1:38
    and the next one I'm going to show you
  • 1:38 - 1:39
    are pretty much the biggestpsychological gender differences
  • 1:39 - 1:41
    ever discovered in psychology.
  • 1:41 - 1:42
    So here's the next one.
  • 1:42 - 1:45
    Any psychologist will tell youthat women are better
  • 1:45 - 1:46
    with language and grammar than men.
  • 1:46 - 1:49
    So here's performanceon the standardized grammar test.
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    There go the women. There go to the men.
  • 1:51 - 1:53
    Again, yes, women are better on average,
  • 1:53 - 1:55
    but the lines are so close
  • 1:55 - 1:56
    that 33 percent of men
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    are better than the average woman,
  • 1:58 - 2:00
    and again, if it was 50 percent,
  • 2:00 - 2:02
    that would representcomplete gender equality.
  • 2:02 - 2:05
    So it's not reallya case of Mars and Venus.
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    It's more a case of, if anything,Mars and Snickers:
  • 2:07 - 2:09
    basically the same, but, you know,
  • 2:09 - 2:12
    one's maybe slightlynuttier than the other.
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    I won't say which.
  • 2:14 - 2:16
    Right. Now we've got you warmed up.
  • 2:16 - 2:19
    Let's psychoanalyze you usingthe famous Rorschach inkblot test.
  • 2:19 - 2:22
    So you can probably see two, I dunno,two bears or two people or something.
  • 2:22 - 2:24
    But what do you think they're doing?
  • 2:24 - 2:27
    Put your hand up if you thinkthey're saying hello.
  • 2:27 - 2:29
    Not many people. Okay.
  • 2:29 - 2:31
    Put your hands up if you thinkthey are high-fiving.
  • 2:31 - 2:33
    Okay. What if you think they're fighting?
  • 2:33 - 2:34
    Only a few people there.
  • 2:34 - 2:38
    Okay, so if you think they'resaying hello or high-fiving,
  • 2:38 - 2:40
    then that means you're a friendly person.
  • 2:40 - 2:42
    If you think they're fighting,that means you're a bit more
  • 2:42 - 2:44
    of a nasty, aggressive person.
  • 2:44 - 2:46
    Are you a lover or a fighter, basically.
  • 2:46 - 2:48
    What about this one?This isn't really a voting one,
  • 2:48 - 2:51
    so on three, everyoneshout out what you see.
  • 2:51 - 2:54
    One, two, three.
  • 2:54 - 2:55
    I heard hamster. Who said hamster?
  • 2:55 - 2:57
    That was very worrying.
  • 2:57 - 2:58
    A guy there said hamster.
  • 2:58 - 3:02
    Well, you should seesome kind of two-legged animal here,
  • 3:02 - 3:04
    and then the mirror image of them there.
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    If you didn't, then this meansthat you have difficulty
  • 3:07 - 3:09
    processing complex situations
  • 3:09 - 3:12
    where there's a lot going on.
  • 3:12 - 3:14
    Except, of course,it doesn't mean that at all.
  • 3:14 - 3:16
    Rorschach inkblot testshave basically no validity
  • 3:16 - 3:19
    when it comes to diagnosingpeople's personality
  • 3:19 - 3:21
    and are not usedby modern-day psychologists.
  • 3:21 - 3:23
    In fact, one recent study found
  • 3:23 - 3:26
    that when you do tryto diagnose people's personality
  • 3:26 - 3:28
    using Rorschach inkblot tests,
  • 3:28 - 3:29
    schizophrenia was diagnosed
  • 3:29 - 3:33
    in about one sixth of apparentlyperfectly normal participants.
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    So if you didn't do that well on this,
  • 3:36 - 3:39
    maybe you are nota very visual type of person.
  • 3:39 - 3:41
    So let's do anotherquick quiz to find out.
  • 3:41 - 3:43
    When making a cake, do you prefer to
  • 3:43 - 3:45
    -- so hands up for each one again --
  • 3:45 - 3:48
    do you prefer to usea recipe book with pictures?
  • 3:48 - 3:50
    Yeah, a few people.
  • 3:50 - 3:52
    Have a friend talk you through?
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    Or have a go, making it upas you go along?
  • 3:55 - 3:57
    Quite a few people there.
  • 3:57 - 3:58
    Okay, so if you said a,
  • 3:58 - 4:00
    then this means that youare a visual learner
  • 4:00 - 4:02
    and you learn best when information
  • 4:02 - 4:04
    is presented in a visual style.
  • 4:04 - 4:07
    If you said b, it meansyou're an auditory learner,
  • 4:07 - 4:10
    that you learn best when informationis presented to you in an auditory format,
  • 4:10 - 4:11
    and if you said c,
  • 4:11 - 4:13
    it means that you'rea kinesthetic learner,
  • 4:13 - 4:15
    that you learn best when you get stuck in
  • 4:15 - 4:17
    and do things with your hands.
  • 4:17 - 4:19
    Except, of course,as you've probably guessed,
  • 4:19 - 4:22
    that it doesn't, becausethe whole thing is a complete myth.
  • 4:22 - 4:23
    Learning styles are made up
  • 4:23 - 4:26
    and are not supportedby scientific evidence.
  • 4:26 - 4:29
    So we know this because intightly controlled experimental studies,
  • 4:29 - 4:31
    when learners are given material to learn
  • 4:31 - 4:34
    either in their preferred styleor an opposite style,
  • 4:34 - 4:35
    it makes no difference at all
  • 4:35 - 4:37
    to the amount of informationthat they retain.
  • 4:37 - 4:40
    And if you think about itfor just a second,
  • 4:40 - 4:42
    it's just obviousthat this has to be true.
  • 4:42 - 4:44
    It's obvious thatthe best presentation format
  • 4:44 - 4:46
    depends not on you,
  • 4:46 - 4:47
    but on what you're trying to learn.
  • 4:47 - 4:49
    Could you learn to drive a car,for example,
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    just by listening to someonetelling you what to do
  • 4:52 - 4:54
    with no kinesthetic experience?
  • 4:54 - 4:55
    Could you solve simultaneous equations
  • 4:55 - 4:58
    by talking them through in your headand without writing them down?
  • 4:58 - 5:00
    Could you ?? for your architecture exams
  • 5:00 - 5:03
    using interpretive danceif you're a kinesthetic learner?
  • 5:03 - 5:05
    No. What you need to do is match
  • 5:05 - 5:09
    the material to be learnedto the presentation format,
  • 5:09 - 5:10
    not you.
  • 5:10 - 5:12
    So, I know many of youare A-level students
  • 5:12 - 5:14
    that will have recently gottenyour ???? results.
  • 5:14 - 5:17
    And if you didn't quite getwhat you were hoping for,
  • 5:17 - 5:19
    then you can't really blameyour learning style,
  • 5:19 - 5:23
    but one thing that you might wantto think about blaming is your genes.
  • 5:23 - 5:24
    So what this is all about
  • 5:24 - 5:27
    is a recent studyat University College London
  • 5:27 - 5:29
    found that 58 percent of the variation
  • 5:29 - 5:32
    between different studentsand their ??? results
  • 5:32 - 5:34
    was down to genetic factors.
  • 5:34 - 5:36
    So that sounds a very precise figure,
  • 5:36 - 5:37
    so how can we tell?
  • 5:37 - 5:41
    Well, when we want to unpackthe relative contributions
  • 5:41 - 5:43
    of genes and the environment,
  • 5:43 - 5:45
    what we can do is do a twin study.
  • 5:45 - 5:49
    So identical twins share100 percent of their environment
  • 5:49 - 5:51
    and 100 percent of their genes,
  • 5:51 - 5:54
    whereas non-identical twinsshare 100 percent of their environment,
  • 5:54 - 5:56
    but just like any brother and sister,
  • 5:56 - 5:57
    share only 50 percent of their genes.
  • 5:57 - 6:01
    So by comparing how similar??? results are in identical twins
  • 6:01 - 6:04
    versus non-identical twins,
  • 6:04 - 6:05
    and doing some clever math,
  • 6:05 - 6:09
    we can an idea of how much variationand performance is due to the environment
  • 6:09 - 6:11
    and how much is due to genes.
  • 6:11 - 6:15
    And it turns out that it'sabout 58 percent due to genes.
  • 6:15 - 6:19
    So this isn't to undermine the hard workthat you and your teachers here put in.
  • 6:19 - 6:22
    If you didn't quite get the ??? resultsthat you were hoping for,
  • 6:22 - 6:27
    then you can always try blamingyour parents, or at least their genes.
  • 6:27 - 6:29
    One thing that you shouldn't blame
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    is being a left brainedor right brained learner,
  • 6:31 - 6:33
    because again, this is a myth.
  • 6:33 - 6:36
    So the myth here is thatthe left brain is logical,
  • 6:36 - 6:37
    it's good with equations like this,
  • 6:37 - 6:39
    and the right brain is more creative,
  • 6:39 - 6:42
    so the right brain is better at music.
  • 6:42 - 6:44
    But again, this is a mythbecause nearly everything that you do
  • 6:44 - 6:47
    involves nearly all partsof your brain talking together,
  • 6:47 - 6:51
    even just the most mundane thinglike having a normal conversation.
  • 6:51 - 6:54
    However, perhaps one reasonwhy this myth has survived
  • 6:54 - 6:56
    is that there isa slight grain of truth to it.
  • 6:56 - 6:58
    So a related version of the myth
  • 6:58 - 7:01
    is that left-handed people aremore creative than right-handed people,
  • 7:01 - 7:05
    which kind of makes sense becauseyour brain controls the opposite hands,
  • 7:05 - 7:06
    so left-handed people,
  • 7:06 - 7:08
    the right side of the brainis slightly more active
  • 7:08 - 7:10
    than the left hand side of the brain,
  • 7:10 - 7:13
    and the idea is the right-hand sideis more creative.
  • 7:13 - 7:14
    Now, it isn't true per se
  • 7:14 - 7:17
    that left-handed people are more creativethan right-handed people.
  • 7:17 - 7:20
    What is true that ambidextrous people,
  • 7:20 - 7:22
    or people who use both handsfor different tasks,
  • 7:22 - 7:26
    are more creative thinkersthan one-handed people,
  • 7:26 - 7:28
    because being ambidextrous involves
  • 7:28 - 7:31
    having both sides of the braintalk to each other a lot,
  • 7:31 - 7:34
    which seems to be involvedin creating flexible thinking.
  • 7:34 - 7:36
    The myth of the creative left-hander
  • 7:36 - 7:38
    arises from the factthat being ambidextrous
  • 7:38 - 7:40
    is more common amongst left-handers
  • 7:40 - 7:41
    than right handers,
  • 7:41 - 7:44
    so a grain of truth in the ideaof the creative left-hander,
  • 7:44 - 7:46
    but not much.
  • 7:46 - 7:48
    A related myth that you'veprobably heard of
  • 7:48 - 7:51
    is that we only use10 percent of our brains.
  • 7:51 - 7:52
    This is, again, a complete myth.
  • 7:52 - 7:55
    Nearly everything that we do,even the most mundane thing,
  • 7:55 - 7:57
    uses nearly all of our brains.
  • 7:57 - 8:01
    That said, it is of course true
  • 8:01 - 8:03
    that most of us don't use our brainpower
  • 8:03 - 8:05
    quite as well as we could.
  • 8:05 - 8:08
    So what could we doto boost our brain power?
  • 8:08 - 8:10
    Maybe we could listento a nice bit of Mozart.
  • 8:10 - 8:13
    So have you heard of the ideaof the Mozart effect?
  • 8:13 - 8:15
    So the idea is that listening to Mozart
  • 8:15 - 8:18
    makes you smarter and improvesyour performance on IQ tests.
  • 8:18 - 8:20
    Now again, what's interestingabout this myth
  • 8:20 - 8:22
    is that although it's basically a myth,
  • 8:22 - 8:24
    there is a grain of truth to it.
  • 8:24 - 8:25
    So the original study found that
  • 8:25 - 8:29
    participants who were playedMozart music for a few minutes
  • 8:29 - 8:32
    did better on a subsequent IQ test
  • 8:32 - 8:35
    than participants who simplysat in silence.
  • 8:35 - 8:39
    But a follow-up study recruitedsome people who liked Mozart music
  • 8:39 - 8:41
    and then another group of people
  • 8:41 - 8:43
    who were fans of the horror storiesof Stephen King.
  • 8:43 - 8:47
    And they played the peoplethe music or the stories.
  • 8:47 - 8:49
    The people who preferredMozart music to the stories
  • 8:49 - 8:52
    got a bigger IQ boostfrom the Mozart than the stories,
  • 8:52 - 8:55
    but the people who preferredthe stories to the Mozart music
  • 8:55 - 8:56
    got a bigger IQ boost
  • 8:56 - 8:59
    from listening to the Stephen King storiesthan the Mozart music.
  • 8:59 - 9:02
    So the truth is that listeningto something that you enjoy
  • 9:02 - 9:05
    perks you up a bitand gives you a temporary IQ boost
  • 9:05 - 9:07
    on a narrow range of tasks.
  • 9:07 - 9:09
    There's no suggestion thatlistening to Mozart
  • 9:09 - 9:11
    or indeed Stephen King stories
  • 9:11 - 9:15
    is going to make you any smarterin the long run.
  • 9:15 - 9:17
    So another version of the Mozart myth
  • 9:17 - 9:22
    is that listening to Mozart can make younot only cleverer but healthier, too.
  • 9:22 - 9:24
    Unfortunately, this doesn'tseem to be true
  • 9:24 - 9:27
    of someone who listenedto the music of Mozart almost every day,
  • 9:27 - 9:29
    Mozart himself,
  • 9:29 - 9:32
    who suffered from gonorrhea,smallpox, arthritis,
  • 9:32 - 9:35
    and what most people thinkeventually killed him in the end,
  • 9:35 - 9:37
    syphilis.
  • 9:37 - 9:40
    This suggests that Mozartshould have bit more careful, perhaps,
  • 9:40 - 9:43
    when choosing his sexual partners.
  • 9:43 - 9:45
    But how do we choose a partner?
  • 9:45 - 9:48
    So a myth, but I have to sayis sometimes spread a bit by sociologists
  • 9:48 - 9:52
    is that our preferencesin a romantic partner
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    are a product of our culture,
  • 9:54 - 9:56
    that they're very culturally specific,
  • 9:56 - 9:57
    but in fact, the data don't back this up.
  • 9:57 - 10:02
    So a famous study surveyed people from32 different cultures across the globe,
  • 10:02 - 10:03
    from Americans to Zulus,
  • 10:03 - 10:05
    on what they look for in a partner.
  • 10:05 - 10:08
    And in every single cultureacross the globe,
  • 10:08 - 10:12
    men placed more valueon physical attractiveness in a partner
  • 10:12 - 10:13
    than did women,
  • 10:13 - 10:15
    and in every single culture, too,
  • 10:15 - 10:17
    women placed more importance than did men
  • 10:17 - 10:19
    on ambition and high earning power.
  • 10:19 - 10:21
    In every culture, too,
  • 10:21 - 10:23
    men preferred womenwho were younger than themselves,
  • 10:23 - 10:26
    an average of I think it was 2.66 years,
  • 10:26 - 10:28
    and in every culture, too,
  • 10:28 - 10:30
    women preferred menwho were older than them,
  • 10:30 - 10:33
    so an average of 3.42 years,
  • 10:33 - 10:37
    which is why we've got here"Everybody Needs A Sugar Daddy."
  • 10:37 - 10:39
    So moving on from tryingto score with a partner
  • 10:39 - 10:43
    to trying to score in basketballor football or whatever your sport is.
  • 10:43 - 10:45
    So the myth here is that sportsmen
  • 10:45 - 10:47
    go through hot hand streaks,Americans call them,
  • 10:47 - 10:50
    or purple patches,we sometimes say in England,
  • 10:50 - 10:51
    where they just can't miss,
  • 10:51 - 10:52
    like this guy here.
  • 10:52 - 10:56
    But in fact, what happens is thatif you analyze the pattern
  • 10:56 - 10:58
    of hits and misses statistically,
  • 10:58 - 11:00
    it turns out that it'snearly always at random.
  • 11:00 - 11:03
    Your brain creates patternsfrom the randomness.
  • 11:03 - 11:04
    So if you toss a coin, you know,
  • 11:04 - 11:08
    a streak of heads or tails is goingto come out somewhere in randomness,
  • 11:08 - 11:11
    and becomes the brain likesto see patterns where there are none,
  • 11:11 - 11:14
    we look at these streaksand attribute meanings to them
  • 11:14 - 11:16
    and say, "Yeah he's really on form today,"
  • 11:16 - 11:18
    whereas actually you wouldget the same pattern
  • 11:18 - 11:21
    if you were just gettinghits and misses at random.
  • 11:21 - 11:25
    So an exception to this, however,is penalty shootouts.
  • 11:25 - 11:28
    A recent study lookingat penalty shootouts in football
  • 11:28 - 11:30
    shows that players who represent countries
  • 11:30 - 11:33
    with a very bad recordin penalty shootouts,
  • 11:33 - 11:35
    like, for example, England,
  • 11:35 - 11:39
    tend to be quicker to take their shotsthan countries with a better record,
  • 11:39 - 11:42
    and presumably as a result,they're more likely to miss.
  • 11:42 - 11:44
    Which raises the question
  • 11:44 - 11:47
    of if there's any way that wecould improve people's performance,
  • 11:47 - 11:49
    and one thing you might think about doing
  • 11:49 - 11:51
    is punishing people for their misses
  • 11:51 - 11:53
    and seeing if that improves things.
  • 11:53 - 11:56
    This idea, the effect that punishmentcan improve performance,
  • 11:56 - 11:58
    is what participantsthought they were testing
  • 11:58 - 12:01
    in Milgram's famous learningand punishment experiment
  • 12:01 - 12:04
    that you've probably heard aboutif you're a psychology student.
  • 12:04 - 12:07
    The story goes that participantswere prepared to give
  • 12:07 - 12:10
    what they believed to be fatalelectric shocks to a fellow participant
  • 12:10 - 12:12
    when they got a question wrong,
  • 12:12 - 12:15
    just because someonein a white coat told them too.
  • 12:15 - 12:17
    But this story is a mythfor three reasons.
  • 12:17 - 12:20
    Firstly and most crucially,the lab coat wasn't white.
  • 12:20 - 12:22
    It was, in fact, grey.
  • 12:22 - 12:26
    Secondly, the participantswere told before the study
  • 12:26 - 12:29
    and reminded any timethey raised a concern,
  • 12:29 - 12:32
    that although the shocks were painful,they were not fatal
  • 12:32 - 12:35
    and indeed causedno permanent damage whatsoever.
  • 12:35 - 12:37
    And thirdly, participantsdidn't give the shocks
  • 12:37 - 12:39
    just because someonein the coat told them to.
  • 12:39 - 12:42
    When they were interviewedafter the study,
  • 12:42 - 12:44
    all the participants saidthat they firmly believed
  • 12:44 - 12:48
    that the learning and punishment studyserved a worthy scientific purpose
  • 12:48 - 12:50
    which would haveenduring gains for science
  • 12:50 - 12:54
    as opposed to the momentarynon-fatal discomfort
  • 12:54 - 12:57
    caused to the participants.
  • 12:57 - 13:00
    Okay, so I've been talkingfor about 12 minutes now,
  • 13:00 - 13:01
    and you've probably been sitting there
  • 13:01 - 13:05
    listening to me, analyzingmy speech patterns and body language
  • 13:05 - 13:08
    and trying to work out if you shouldtake any notice of what I'm saying,
  • 13:08 - 13:11
    whether I'm telling the truthor whether I'm lying,
  • 13:11 - 13:13
    but if so you'veprobably completely failed,
  • 13:13 - 13:15
    because although we all thinkwe can catch a liar
  • 13:15 - 13:17
    from their body languageand speech patterns,
  • 13:17 - 13:20
    hundreds of psychological testsover the years have shown
  • 13:20 - 13:23
    that all of us, includingpolice officers and detectives,
  • 13:23 - 13:26
    are basically at chance when it comesto detecting lies from body language
  • 13:26 - 13:28
    and verbal patterns.
  • 13:28 - 13:30
    Interestingly, there is one exception:
  • 13:30 - 13:32
    TV appeals for missing relatives.
  • 13:32 - 13:35
    It's quite easy to predictwhen the relatives are missing
  • 13:35 - 13:38
    and when the appealers have in factmurdered the relatives themselves.
  • 13:38 - 13:42
    So hoax appealers are more likelyto shake their heads, to look away,
  • 13:42 - 13:43
    and to make errors in their speech,
  • 13:43 - 13:45
    whereas genuine appealers are more likely
  • 13:45 - 13:48
    to express hope that the personwill return safely
  • 13:48 - 13:50
    and to avoid brutal language.
  • 13:50 - 13:54
    So, for example, they might say"taken from us" rather than "killed."
  • 13:54 - 13:55
    Speaking of which,
  • 13:55 - 13:57
    it's about time I killed this talk,
  • 13:57 - 13:59
    but before I do, I just want to give you
  • 13:59 - 14:00
    in 30 seconds
  • 14:00 - 14:03
    the overarching myth of psychology.
  • 14:03 - 14:06
    So the myth is that psychology
  • 14:06 - 14:08
    is just a collectionof interesting theories,
  • 14:08 - 14:09
    all of which say something useful
  • 14:09 - 14:11
    and all of which have something to offer.
  • 14:11 - 14:14
    What I hope to have shown youin the past few minutes
  • 14:14 - 14:15
    is that this isn't true.
  • 14:15 - 14:19
    What we need to do is assesspsychological theories
  • 14:19 - 14:20
    by seeing what predictions they make,
  • 14:20 - 14:23
    whether that is that listening to Mozartmakes you smarter,
  • 14:23 - 14:26
    that you learn better when information
  • 14:26 - 14:28
    is presented in yourpreferred learning style,
  • 14:28 - 14:32
    or whatever it is, all of theseare testable empirical predictions,
  • 14:32 - 14:33
    and the only way we can make progress
  • 14:33 - 14:35
    is to test these predictionsagainst the data
  • 14:35 - 14:38
    in tightly controlledexperimental studies,
  • 14:38 - 14:41
    and it's only by doing sothat we can hope to discover
  • 14:41 - 14:44
    which of these theoriesare well-supported,
  • 14:44 - 14:47
    and which, like all the onesI've told you about today, are myths.
  • 14:47 - 14:48
    Thank you.
  • 14:48 - 14:52
    (Applause)
Title:
The top 10 myths of psychology | Ben Ambridge | TEDxYouth@Manchester
Description:

This talk was given at a local TEDx event, produced independently of the TED Conferences.
How much of what you think about your brain is actually wrong? In this whistlestop tour of dis-proved science, Ben Ambridge walks through 10 popular ideas about psychology that have been proven wrong — and uncovers a few surprising truths about how our brains really work.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
15:23

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions