Return to Video

Is there a reproducibility crisis in science? - Matt Anticole

  • 0:07 - 0:11
    In 2011, a team of physicists reported
    a startling discovery -
  • 0:11 - 0:14
    neutrinos traveled faster
    than the speed of light
  • 0:14 - 0:16
    by 60 billionths of a second
  • 0:16 - 0:21
    in their 730 kilometer trip from Geneva
    to a detector in Italy.
  • 0:21 - 0:25
    Despite six months of double checking,
    the bizarre discovery refused to yield.
  • 0:25 - 0:28
    But rather than celebrating
    a physics revolution,
  • 0:28 - 0:30
    the researchers published a cautious paper
  • 0:30 - 0:35
    arguing for continued research in an
    effort to explain the observed anomaly.
  • 0:35 - 0:41
    In time, the error was tracked to a single
    incorrectly connected fiber optic cable.
  • 0:41 - 0:46
    This example reminds us that real
    science is more than static textbooks.
  • 0:46 - 0:50
    Instead, researchers around the world
    are continuously publishing
  • 0:50 - 0:51
    their latest discoveries
  • 0:51 - 0:55
    with each paper adding
    to the scientific conversation.
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    Published studies
    can motivate future research,
  • 0:57 - 0:59
    inspire new products,
  • 0:59 - 1:01
    and inform government policy.
  • 1:01 - 1:05
    So it's important that we have confidence
    in the published results.
  • 1:05 - 1:07
    If their conclusions are wrong,
  • 1:07 - 1:08
    we risk time,
  • 1:08 - 1:09
    resources,
  • 1:09 - 1:12
    and even our health in the pursuit
    of false leads.
  • 1:12 - 1:14
    When findings are significant,
  • 1:14 - 1:17
    they are frequently double-checked
    by other researchers,
  • 1:17 - 1:19
    either by reanalyzing the data
  • 1:19 - 1:22
    or by redoing the entire experiment.
  • 1:22 - 1:25
    For example, it took repeated
    investigation of the CERN data
  • 1:25 - 1:29
    before the timing era was tracked down.
  • 1:29 - 1:33
    Unfortunately, there are currently neither
    the resources nor professional incentives
  • 1:33 - 1:39
    to double-check the more than 1 million
    scientific papers published annually.
  • 1:39 - 1:43
    Even when papers are challenged,
    the results are not reassuring.
  • 1:43 - 1:46
    Recent studies that examined dozens
    of published pharmaceutical papers
  • 1:46 - 1:51
    managed to replicate the results of
    less than 25% of them.
  • 1:51 - 1:55
    And similar results have been found
    in other scientific disciplines.
  • 1:55 - 1:58
    There are a variety of sources
    for irreproducible results.
  • 1:58 - 2:04
    Errors could hide in their original
    design, execution or analysis of the data.
  • 2:04 - 2:05
    Unknown factors,
  • 2:05 - 2:08
    such as patients' undisclosed condition
    in a medical study,
  • 2:08 - 2:12
    can produce results that are
    not repeatable in new test subjects.
  • 2:12 - 2:16
    And sometimes, the second research group
    can't reproduce the original results
  • 2:16 - 2:20
    simply because they don't know
    exactly what the original group did.
  • 2:20 - 2:24
    However, some problems might stem
    from systematic decisions
  • 2:24 - 2:26
    in how we do science.
  • 2:26 - 2:27
    Researchers,
  • 2:27 - 2:29
    the institutions that employ them,
  • 2:29 - 2:31
    and the scientific journals
    that publish findings
  • 2:31 - 2:35
    are expected to produce
    big results frequently.
  • 2:35 - 2:37
    Important papers can advance careers,
  • 2:37 - 2:39
    generate media interest,
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    and secure essential funding,
  • 2:41 - 2:45
    so there's slim motivation for researchers
    to challenge their own exciting results.
  • 2:45 - 2:48
    In addition, little incentive exists
  • 2:48 - 2:52
    to publish results unsupportive
    of the expected hypothesis.
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    That results in a deluge of agreement
    between what was expected
  • 2:55 - 2:57
    and what was found.
  • 2:57 - 3:00
    In rare occasions, this can even lead
    to deliberate fabrication,
  • 3:00 - 3:05
    such as in 2013, when a researcher
    spiked rabbit blood with human blood
  • 3:05 - 3:09
    to give false evidence that
    his HIV vaccine was working.
  • 3:09 - 3:11
    The publish or perish mindset
  • 3:11 - 3:15
    can also compromise academic journals'
    traditional peer-review processes
  • 3:15 - 3:17
    which are safety checks
  • 3:17 - 3:20
    where experts examine submitted papers
    for potential shortcomings.
  • 3:20 - 3:22
    The current system,
  • 3:22 - 3:24
    which might involve only one
    or two reviewers,
  • 3:24 - 3:26
    can be woefully ineffective.
  • 3:26 - 3:29
    That was demonstrated in a 1998 study
  • 3:29 - 3:33
    where eight weaknesses were deliberately
    inserted into papers,
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    but only around 25%
    were caught upon review.
  • 3:36 - 3:41
    Many scientists are working toward
    improving reproducibility in their fields.
  • 3:41 - 3:43
    There's a push to make researchers
    raw data,
  • 3:43 - 3:45
    experimental procedures,
  • 3:45 - 3:48
    and analytical techniques
    more openly available
  • 3:48 - 3:51
    in order to ease replication efforts.
  • 3:51 - 3:53
    The peer review process can also
    be strengthened
  • 3:53 - 3:57
    to more efficiently weed out weak papers
    prior to publication.
  • 3:57 - 4:00
    And we could temper the pressure
    to find big results
  • 4:00 - 4:04
    by publishing more papers that fail
    to confirm the original hypothesis,
  • 4:04 - 4:09
    an even that happens far more than
    current scientific literature suggests.
  • 4:09 - 4:12
    Science always has, and always will,
    encounter some false starts
  • 4:12 - 4:15
    as part of the collective acquisition
    of new knowledge.
  • 4:15 - 4:18
    Finding ways to improve
    the reproducibility of our results
  • 4:18 - 4:22
    can help us weed out those false starts
    more effectively,
  • 4:22 - 4:25
    keeping us moving steadily toward
    exciting new discoveries.
Title:
Is there a reproducibility crisis in science? - Matt Anticole
Description:

View full lesson: http://ed.ted.com/lessons/scientific-irreproducibility-matthew-anticole

Lesson by Matthew Anticole, animation by Brett Underhill

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:47

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions