Return to Video

Sometimes it's good to give up the driver's seat

  • 0:01 - 0:04
    I want to start on a slightly somber note.
  • 0:04 - 0:07
    Two thousand and seven, five years ago,
  • 0:07 - 0:10
    my wife gets diagnosed with breast cancer,
  • 0:10 - 0:14
    stage IIB.
  • 0:14 - 0:16
    Now, looking back, the most harrowing
  • 0:16 - 0:17
    part of that experience
  • 0:17 - 0:19
    was not just the hospital visits --
  • 0:19 - 0:22
    these were very painful for my wife, understandably so.
  • 0:22 - 0:24
    It was not even the initial shock of knowing
  • 0:24 - 0:26
    that she had breast cancer, just 39 years old,
  • 0:26 - 0:30
    absolutely no history of cancer in her family.
  • 0:30 - 0:33
    The most horrifying and agonizing part
  • 0:33 - 0:36
    of the whole experience was we were making
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    decisions after decisions after decisions
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    that were being thrust upon us.
  • 0:40 - 0:43
    Should it be a mastectomy? Should it be a lumpectomy?
  • 0:43 - 0:45
    Should it be a more aggressive form of treatment,
  • 0:45 - 0:47
    given that it was stage IIB?
  • 0:47 - 0:49
    With all the side effects?
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    Or should it be a less aggressive form of treatment?
  • 0:52 - 0:54
    And these were being pressed upon us
  • 0:54 - 0:56
    by the doctors.
  • 0:56 - 0:58
    Now, you could ask this question,
  • 0:58 - 0:59
    why were the doctors doing this?
  • 0:59 - 1:01
    Now, a simplistic answer would be,
  • 1:01 - 1:05
    the doctors are doing this because they want to protect themselves legally.
  • 1:05 - 1:07
    I think that is too simplistic.
  • 1:07 - 1:09
    These are well-meaning doctors,
  • 1:09 - 1:11
    some of them have gone on to become very good friends.
  • 1:11 - 1:13
    They probably were simply following the wisdom
  • 1:13 - 1:17
    that has come down the ages, this adage that when you're making decisions,
  • 1:17 - 1:19
    especially decisions of importance,
  • 1:19 - 1:22
    it's best to be in charge, it's best to be in control,
  • 1:22 - 1:25
    it's best to be in the driver's seat.
  • 1:25 - 1:27
    And we were certainly in the driver's seat,
  • 1:27 - 1:29
    making all these decisions, and let me tell you,
  • 1:29 - 1:30
    if some of you had been there,
  • 1:30 - 1:34
    it was a most agonizing and harrowing experience.
  • 1:34 - 1:36
    Which got me thinking.
  • 1:36 - 1:37
    I said, is there any validity to
  • 1:37 - 1:41
    this whole adage that when you're making decisions,
  • 1:41 - 1:43
    it's best to take the driver's seat,
  • 1:43 - 1:45
    be in charge, be in control?
  • 1:45 - 1:48
    Or are there contexts where we're far better off
  • 1:48 - 1:51
    taking the passenger's seat and have someone else drive?
  • 1:51 - 1:53
    For example, a trusted financial advisor,
  • 1:53 - 1:56
    could be a trusted doctor, etc.
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    And since I study human decision making,
  • 1:58 - 2:01
    I said, I'm going to run some studies
  • 2:01 - 2:03
    to find some answers.
  • 2:03 - 2:05
    And I'm going to share one of these studies with you today.
  • 2:05 - 2:09
    So, imagine that all of you are participants in the study.
  • 2:09 - 2:12
    I want to tell you that what you're going to do in the study is
  • 2:12 - 2:15
    you're going to drink a cup of tea.
  • 2:15 - 2:19
    If you're wondering why, I'll tell you why in a few seconds from now.
  • 2:19 - 2:22
    You are going to solve a series of puzzles,
  • 2:22 - 2:26
    and I'm going to show you examples of these puzzles momentarily.
  • 2:26 - 2:27
    And the more puzzles you solve,
  • 2:27 - 2:31
    the greater the chances that you'll win some prizes.
  • 2:31 - 2:33
    Now, why do you have to consume the tea?
  • 2:33 - 2:34
    Why? Because it makes a lot of sense.
  • 2:34 - 2:37
    In order to solve these puzzles effectively,
  • 2:37 - 2:41
    if you think about it, your mind needs to be in two states simultaneously.
  • 2:41 - 2:44
    Right? It needs to be alert,
  • 2:44 - 2:47
    for which caffeine is very good.
  • 2:47 - 2:50
    Simultaneously, it needs to be calm.
  • 2:50 - 2:55
    Not agitated, calm. For which chamomile is very good.
  • 2:55 - 2:58
    Now comes the between-subjects design,
  • 2:58 - 3:00
    the AB design, the AB testing.
  • 3:00 - 3:01
    So what I'm going to do is randomly assign you
  • 3:01 - 3:03
    to one of two groups.
  • 3:03 - 3:06
    So imagine that there is an imaginary line out here,
  • 3:06 - 3:09
    so everyone here will be group A,
  • 3:09 - 3:12
    everyone out here will be group B.
  • 3:12 - 3:15
    Now, for you folks, what I'm going to do is
  • 3:15 - 3:17
    I'm going to show you these two teas,
  • 3:17 - 3:19
    and I'm asking you, I'll go ahead and ask you,
  • 3:19 - 3:22
    to choose your tea. So you can choose which of the two tea you want.
  • 3:22 - 3:24
    You can decide, what is your mental state:
  • 3:24 - 3:26
    Okay, I'm going to choose the caffeinated tea,
  • 3:26 - 3:27
    I'm going to choose the chamomile tea.
  • 3:27 - 3:29
    So you're going to be in charge,
  • 3:29 - 3:33
    you're going to be in control, you're going to be in the driver's seat.
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    You folks, I'm going to show you these two teas,
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    but you don't have a choice.
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    I'm going to give you one of these two teas,
  • 3:40 - 3:42
    and keep in mind, I'm going to pick one of these
  • 3:42 - 3:44
    two teas at random for you.
  • 3:44 - 3:46
    And you know that.
  • 3:46 - 3:49
    So if you think about it, this is an extreme case scenario,
  • 3:49 - 3:50
    because in the real world,
  • 3:50 - 3:52
    whenever you are taking passenger's seat,
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    very often the driver is going to be someone you trust,
  • 3:55 - 3:59
    an expert, etc. So this is an extreme case scenario.
  • 3:59 - 4:04
    Now, you're all going to consume the tea.
  • 4:04 - 4:05
    So imagine that you are taking the tea now,
  • 4:05 - 4:07
    we'll wait for you to finish the tea.
  • 4:07 - 4:11
    We'll give another five minutes for the ingredient to have its effects.
  • 4:11 - 4:17
    Now you're going to have 30 minutes to solve 15 puzzles.
  • 4:17 - 4:20
    Here's an example of the puzzle you're going to solve.
  • 4:20 - 4:22
    Anyone in the audience want to take a stab?
  • 4:22 - 4:24
    (Audience: Pulpit.) Baba Shiv: Whoa!
  • 4:24 - 4:26
    Okay, that's cool.
  • 4:26 - 4:28
    Yeah, so what we do if we had you, who will get the answer,
  • 4:28 - 4:33
    as a participant, we would have calibrated the difficulty level
  • 4:33 - 4:35
    of the puzzles to your expertise.
  • 4:35 - 4:37
    Because we want these puzzles to be difficult.
  • 4:37 - 4:39
    These are tricky puzzles because your first instinct
  • 4:39 - 4:44
    is to say "tulip," and then you have to unstick yourself.
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    Right? So these have been calibrated to your level of expertise.
  • 4:47 - 4:51
    Because we want this to be difficult, and I'll tell you why momentarily.
  • 4:51 - 4:54
    Now, here's another example.
  • 4:54 - 4:56
    Anyone? It's much more difficult.
  • 4:56 - 4:58
    (Audience: Embark.) BS: Yeah, wow. Okay.
  • 4:58 - 5:00
    So, yeah, so this is again difficult.
  • 5:00 - 5:02
    You will say "kambar," then you will have to go, "maker,"
  • 5:02 - 5:04
    and all that, and then you can unstick yourself.
  • 5:04 - 5:08
    Okay, so you have 30 minutes now to solve these 15 puzzles.
  • 5:08 - 5:11
    Now, the question we're asking here
  • 5:11 - 5:13
    is in terms of the outcome,
  • 5:13 - 5:15
    in terms of the number of puzzles solved,
  • 5:15 - 5:18
    will you in the driver's seat
  • 5:18 - 5:19
    end up solving more puzzles,
  • 5:19 - 5:22
    because you are in control, you could decide which tea you will choose,
  • 5:22 - 5:25
    or would you be better off,
  • 5:25 - 5:27
    in terms of the number of puzzles solved?
  • 5:27 - 5:30
    And systemically what we will show,
  • 5:30 - 5:31
    across a series of studies,
  • 5:31 - 5:34
    is that you, the passengers,
  • 5:34 - 5:37
    even though the tea was picked for you at random,
  • 5:37 - 5:41
    will end up solving more puzzles than you, the drivers.
  • 5:41 - 5:45
    We also observe another thing,
  • 5:45 - 5:47
    and that is, you folks not only are solving fewer puzzles,
  • 5:47 - 5:50
    you're also putting less juice into the task.
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    Less effort, you're less persistent, and so on.
  • 5:53 - 5:54
    How do you know that?
  • 5:54 - 5:56
    Well we have two objective measures.
  • 5:56 - 5:59
    One is, what is the time, on average, you're taking
  • 5:59 - 6:01
    in attempting to solve these puzzles?
  • 6:01 - 6:05
    You will spend less time compared to you.
  • 6:05 - 6:07
    Second, you have 30 minutes to solve these,
  • 6:07 - 6:09
    are you taking the entire 30 minutes, or are you giving up
  • 6:09 - 6:10
    before the 30 minutes elapse?
  • 6:10 - 6:15
    You will be more likely to give up before the 30 minutes elapse compared to you.
  • 6:15 - 6:18
    So, you're putting in less juice, and therefore the outcome:
  • 6:18 - 6:22
    fewer puzzles solved.
  • 6:22 - 6:26
    Now, that brings us now to, why does this happen?
  • 6:26 - 6:30
    And under what situations, when would we see this pattern of results
  • 6:30 - 6:34
    where the passenger is going to show better, more favorable outcomes
  • 6:34 - 6:37
    compared to the driver?
  • 6:37 - 6:42
    It all has to do with when you face what I call the INCA.
  • 6:42 - 6:44
    It's an acronym that stands for
  • 6:44 - 6:48
    the nature of the feedback you're getting after you've made the decision.
  • 6:48 - 6:51
    So, if you think about it, in this particular puzzle task,
  • 6:51 - 6:53
    it could happen in investing in the stock market,
  • 6:53 - 6:55
    very volatile out there, it could be the medical situation --
  • 6:55 - 6:58
    the feedback here is immediate.
  • 6:58 - 7:01
    You know the feedback, whether you're solving the puzzles or not.
  • 7:01 - 7:04
    Right? Second, it is negative.
  • 7:04 - 7:06
    Remember, the deck was stacked against you.
  • 7:06 - 7:09
    In terms of the difficulty level of these puzzles.
  • 7:09 - 7:11
    And this can happen in the medical domain.
  • 7:11 - 7:13
    For example, very early on in the treatment,
  • 7:13 - 7:16
    things are negative, the feedback, before things become positive.
  • 7:16 - 7:18
    Right? It can happen in the stock market.
  • 7:18 - 7:22
    Volatile stock market, getting negative feedback that's also immediate.
  • 7:22 - 7:25
    And the feedback in all these cases is concrete.
  • 7:25 - 7:28
    It's not ambiguous; you know if you've solved the puzzles or not.
  • 7:28 - 7:32
    Now, the added one, apart from this immediacy,
  • 7:32 - 7:35
    negative, this concreteness,
  • 7:35 - 7:39
    now you have a sense of agency.
  • 7:39 - 7:42
    You were responsible for your decision.
  • 7:42 - 7:43
    So what do you do?
  • 7:43 - 7:46
    You focus on the foregone option.
  • 7:46 - 7:50
    You say, you know what? I should have chosen the other tea.
  • 7:50 - 7:53
    (Laughter)
  • 7:53 - 7:56
    That casts your decision in doubt,
  • 7:56 - 7:58
    reduces the confidence you have in the decision,
  • 7:58 - 8:01
    reduces the confidence you have in the performance,
  • 8:01 - 8:02
    the performance in terms of solving the puzzles.
  • 8:02 - 8:05
    And therefore less juice into the task,
  • 8:05 - 8:10
    fewer puzzles solved, a less favorable outcome compared to you folks.
  • 8:10 - 8:12
    And this can happen in the medical domain, if you think about it.
  • 8:12 - 8:14
    Right? A patient in the driver's seat, for example.
  • 8:14 - 8:18
    Less juice, which means keeping herself or himself
  • 8:18 - 8:22
    less physically fit, physically active to hasten the recovery process,
  • 8:22 - 8:26
    which is what is often advocated. You probably wouldn't do that.
  • 8:26 - 8:31
    And therefore, there are times when you're facing the INCA,
  • 8:31 - 8:35
    when the feedback is going to be immediate, negative,
  • 8:35 - 8:37
    concrete, and you have the sense of agency,
  • 8:37 - 8:40
    where you're far better off taking the passenger's seat
  • 8:40 - 8:44
    and have someone else drive.
  • 8:44 - 8:45
    Now, I started off
  • 8:45 - 8:47
    on the somber note.
  • 8:47 - 8:48
    I want to finish up on a more upbeat note.
  • 8:48 - 8:52
    It has now been five years, slightly more than five years,
  • 8:52 - 8:54
    and the good news, thank God,
  • 8:54 - 8:58
    is that the cancer is still in remission.
  • 8:58 - 9:01
    So it all ends well,
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    but one thing I didn't mention was
  • 9:03 - 9:07
    that very early on into her treatment,
  • 9:07 - 9:11
    my wife and I decided that we will take the passenger's seat.
  • 9:11 - 9:13
    And that made so much of a difference
  • 9:13 - 9:16
    in terms of the peace of mind that came with that,
  • 9:16 - 9:17
    we could focus on her recovery.
  • 9:17 - 9:20
    We let the doctors make all the decisions,
  • 9:20 - 9:23
    take the driver's seat.
  • 9:23 - 9:24
    Thank you.
  • 9:24 - 9:27
    (Applause)
Title:
Sometimes it's good to give up the driver's seat
Speaker:
Baba Shiv
Description:

Over the years, research has shown a counterintuitive fact about human nature: That sometimes, having too much choice makes us less happy. This may even be true when it comes to medical treatment. Baba Shiv shares a fascinating study that measures why choice opens the door to doubt, and suggests that ceding control -- especially on life-or-death decisions -- may be the best thing for us.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
09:47

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions