Return to Video

Why we love repetition in music

  • 0:07 - 0:12
    How many times does the chorus repeat
    in your favorite song?
  • 0:12 - 0:17
    And, take a moment to think,
    how many times have you listened to it?
  • 0:17 - 0:22
    Chances are you've heard that chorus
    repeated dozens, if not hundreds, of times,
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    and it's not just popular songs in the West
    that repeat a lot.
  • 0:25 - 0:31
    Repetition is a feature that music from
    cultures around the world tends to share.
  • 0:31 - 0:34
    So, why does music rely
    so heavily on repetition?
  • 0:34 - 0:40
    One part of the answer come from what
    psychologists call the mere-exposure effect.
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    In short, people tend to prefer things
    they've been exposed to before.
  • 0:44 - 0:48
    For example, a song comes on the radio
    that we don't particularly like,
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    but then we hear the song at
    the grocery store, at the movie theater
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    and again on the street corner.
  • 0:54 - 0:56
    Soon, we are tapping to the beat,
    singing the words,
  • 0:56 - 0:59
    even downloading the track.
  • 0:59 - 1:02
    This mere-exposure effect doesn't
    just work for songs.
  • 1:02 - 1:06
    It also works for everything
    from shapes to Super Bowl ads.
  • 1:06 - 1:10
    So, what makes repetition so
    uniquely prevalent in music?
  • 1:10 - 1:15
    To investigate, psychologists asked
    people to listen to musical compositions
  • 1:15 - 1:17
    that avoided exact repetition.
  • 1:17 - 1:20
    They heard excerpts from these pieces
    in either their original form,
  • 1:20 - 1:25
    or in a version that had been digitally
    altered to include repetition.
  • 1:25 - 1:27
    Although the original versions
    had been composed by
  • 1:27 - 1:30
    some of the most respected
    20th century composers,
  • 1:30 - 1:34
    and the repetitive versions had been
    assembled by brute force audio editing,
  • 1:34 - 1:39
    people rated the repetitive versions
    as more enjoyable, more interesting
  • 1:39 - 1:43
    and more likely to have been
    composed by a human artist.
  • 1:43 - 1:46
    Musical repetition is deeply compelling.
  • 1:46 - 1:49
    Think about the Muppets classic,
    "Mahna Mahna."
  • 1:49 - 1:50
    If you've heard it before,
  • 1:50 - 1:53
    it's almost impossible after I sing,
    "Mahna mahna,"
  • 1:53 - 1:57
    not to respond, "Do doo do do do."
  • 1:57 - 1:58
    Repetition connects each bit of music
  • 1:58 - 2:02
    irresistibly to the next bit
    of music that follows it.
  • 2:02 - 2:06
    So when you hear a few notes,
    you're already imagining what's coming next.
  • 2:06 - 2:08
    Your mind is unconsciously singing along,
  • 2:08 - 2:12
    and without noticing,
    you might start humming out loud.
  • 2:12 - 2:15
    Recent studies have shown that when
    people hear a segment of music repeated,
  • 2:15 - 2:18
    they are more likely to move
    or tap along to it.
  • 2:18 - 2:23
    Repetition invites us into music
    as imagined participants,
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    rather than as passive listeners.
  • 2:25 - 2:27
    Research has also shown
  • 2:27 - 2:30
    that listeners shift their attention
    across musical repetitions,
  • 2:30 - 2:34
    focusing on different aspects of
    the sound on each new listen.
  • 2:34 - 2:37
    You might notice the melody
    of a phrase the first time,
  • 2:37 - 2:42
    but when it's repeated, your attention
    shifts to how the guitarist bends a pitch.
  • 2:42 - 2:46
    This also occurs in language,
    with something called semantic satiation.
  • 2:46 - 2:49
    Repeating a word like atlas ad nauseam
  • 2:49 - 2:52
    can make you stop thinking about
    what the word means,
  • 2:52 - 2:57
    and instead focus on the sounds:
    the odd way the "L" follows the "T."
  • 2:57 - 3:00
    In this way, repetition can
    open up new worlds of sound
  • 3:00 - 3:03
    not accessible on first hearing.
  • 3:03 - 3:07
    The "L" following the "T" might not be
    aesthetically relevant to "atlas,"
  • 3:07 - 3:11
    but the guitarist pitch bending
    might be of critical expressive importance.
  • 3:11 - 3:14
    The speech to song illusion
    captures how simply
  • 3:14 - 3:18
    repeating a sentence a number of times
    shifts listeners attention
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    to the pitch and temporal
    aspects of the sound,
  • 3:21 - 3:22
    so that the repeated spoken language
  • 3:22 - 3:26
    actually begins to sound
    like it is being sung.
  • 3:26 - 3:29
    A similar effect happens with
    random sequences of sound.
  • 3:29 - 3:33
    People will rate random sequences
    they've heard on repeated loop
  • 3:33 - 3:38
    as more musical than a random
    sequence they've only heard once.
  • 3:38 - 3:41
    Repetition gives rise to a kind of
    orientation to sound
  • 3:41 - 3:46
    that we think of as distinctively musical,
    where we're listening along with the sound,
  • 3:46 - 3:50
    engaging imaginatively with the note
    about to happen.
  • 3:50 - 3:54
    This mode of listening ties in with our
    susceptibility to musical ear worms,
  • 3:54 - 3:57
    where segments of music
    burrow into our head,
  • 3:57 - 4:00
    and play again and again,
    as if stuck on repeat.
  • 4:00 - 4:03
    Critics are often embarrassed
    by music's repetitiveness,
  • 4:03 - 4:05
    finding it childish or regressive,
  • 4:05 - 4:10
    but repetition, far from an embarrassment,
    is actually a key feature
  • 4:10 - 4:14
    that gives rise to the kind of experience
    we think about as musical.
Title:
Why we love repetition in music
Speaker:
Elizabeth Hellmuth Margulis
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:32

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions