Return to Video

Would you sacrifice one person to save five? - Eleanor Nelsen

  • 0:07 - 0:12
    Imagine you're watching a runaway trolley
    barreling down the tracks
  • 0:12 - 0:16
    straight towards five workers
    who can't escape.
  • 0:16 - 0:18
    You happen to be standing next to a switch
  • 0:18 - 0:22
    that will divert the trolley
    onto a second track.
  • 0:22 - 0:23
    Here's the problem.
  • 0:23 - 0:28
    That track has a worker on it, too,
    but just one.
  • 0:28 - 0:29
    What do you do?
  • 0:29 - 0:33
    Do you sacrifice one person to save five?
  • 0:33 - 0:35
    This is the trolley problem,
  • 0:35 - 0:42
    a version of an ethical dilemma that
    philosopher Philippa Foot devised in 1967.
  • 0:42 - 0:45
    It's popular because it forces us
    to think about how to choose
  • 0:45 - 0:48
    when there are no good choices.
  • 0:48 - 0:50
    Do we pick the action
    with the best outcome
  • 0:50 - 0:55
    or stick to a moral code that prohibits
    causing someone's death?
  • 0:55 - 1:01
    In one survey, about 90% of respondents
    said that it's okay to flip the switch,
  • 1:01 - 1:04
    letting one worker die to save five,
  • 1:04 - 1:09
    and other studies, including a virtual
    reality simulation of the dilemma,
  • 1:09 - 1:11
    have found similar results.
  • 1:11 - 1:16
    These judgments are consistent with the
    philosophical principle of utilitarianism
  • 1:16 - 1:19
    which argues that
    the morally correct decision
  • 1:19 - 1:23
    is the one that maximizes well-being
    for the greatest number of people.
  • 1:23 - 1:25
    The five lives outweigh one,
  • 1:25 - 1:31
    even if achieving that outcome requires
    condemning someone to death.
  • 1:31 - 1:33
    But people don't always take
    the utilitarian view,
  • 1:33 - 1:37
    which we can see by changing
    the trolley problem a bit.
  • 1:37 - 1:40
    This time, you're standing on a bridge
    over the track
  • 1:40 - 1:43
    as the runaway trolley approaches.
  • 1:43 - 1:45
    Now there's no second track,
  • 1:45 - 1:49
    but there is a very large man
    on the bridge next to you.
  • 1:49 - 1:52
    If you push him over,
    his body will stop the trolley,
  • 1:52 - 1:54
    saving the five workers,
  • 1:54 - 1:56
    but he'll die.
  • 1:56 - 1:59
    To utilitarians,
    the decision is exactly the same,
  • 1:59 - 2:02
    lose one life to save five.
  • 2:02 - 2:05
    But in this case, only about 10% of people
  • 2:05 - 2:08
    say that it's OK to throw the man
    onto the tracks.
  • 2:08 - 2:12
    Our instincts tell us that deliberately
    causing someone's death
  • 2:12 - 2:16
    is different than allowing them to die
    as collateral damage.
  • 2:16 - 2:21
    It just feels wrong for reasons
    that are hard to explain.
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    This intersection between ethics
    and psychology
  • 2:23 - 2:27
    is what's so interesting
    about the trolley problem.
  • 2:27 - 2:31
    The dilemma in its many variations reveal
    that what we think is right or wrong
  • 2:31 - 2:36
    depends on factors other than
    a logical weighing of the pros and cons.
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    For example, men are more likely
    than women
  • 2:39 - 2:43
    to say it's okay to push the man
    over the bridge.
  • 2:43 - 2:47
    So are people who watch a comedy clip
    before doing the thought experiment.
  • 2:47 - 2:49
    And in one virtual reality study,
  • 2:49 - 2:53
    people were more willing
    to sacrifice men than women.
  • 2:53 - 2:55
    Researchers have studied
    the brain activity
  • 2:55 - 3:00
    of people thinking through the classic
    and bridge versions.
  • 3:00 - 3:04
    Both scenarios activate areas of the brain
    involved in conscious decision-making
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    and emotional responses.
  • 3:07 - 3:11
    But in the bridge version,
    the emotional response is much stronger.
  • 3:11 - 3:13
    So is activity in an area of the brain
  • 3:13 - 3:17
    associated with processing
    internal conflict.
  • 3:17 - 3:18
    Why the difference?
  • 3:18 - 3:23
    One explanation is that pushing someone
    to their death feels more personal,
  • 3:23 - 3:27
    activating an emotional aversion
    to killing another person,
  • 3:27 - 3:31
    but we feel conflicted because we know
    it's still the logical choice.
  • 3:31 - 3:36
    "Trolleyology" has been criticized by some
    philosophers and psychologists.
  • 3:36 - 3:41
    They argue that it doesn't reveal anything
    because its premise is so unrealistic
  • 3:41 - 3:45
    that study participants
    don't take it seriously.
  • 3:45 - 3:49
    But new technology is making this kind
    of ethical analysis
  • 3:49 - 3:51
    more important than ever.
  • 3:51 - 3:54
    For example, driver-less cars
    may have to handle choices
  • 3:54 - 3:58
    like causing a small accident
    to prevent a larger one.
  • 3:58 - 4:02
    Meanwhile, governments are researching
    autonomous military drones
  • 4:02 - 4:06
    that could wind up making decisions of
    whether they'll risk civilian casualties
  • 4:06 - 4:09
    to attack a high-value target.
  • 4:09 - 4:11
    If we want these actions to be ethical,
  • 4:11 - 4:15
    we have to decide in advance
    how to value human life
  • 4:15 - 4:18
    and judge the greater good.
  • 4:18 - 4:20
    So researchers who study
    autonomous systems
  • 4:20 - 4:22
    are collaborating with philosophers
  • 4:22 - 4:28
    to address the complex problem
    of programming ethics into machines,
  • 4:28 - 4:31
    which goes to show that
    even hypothetical dilemmas
  • 4:31 - 4:35
    can wind up on a collision course
    with the real world.
Title:
Would you sacrifice one person to save five? - Eleanor Nelsen
Speaker:
Eleanor Nelsen
Description:

View full lesson: http://ed.ted.com/lessons/would-you-sacrifice-one-person-to-save-five-eleanor-nelsen

Imagine you’re watching a runaway trolley barreling down the tracks, straight towards five workers. You happen to be standing next to a switch that will divert the trolley onto a second track. Here’s the problem: that track has a worker on it, too — but just one. What do you do? Do you sacrifice one person to save five? Eleanor Nelsen details the ethical dilemma that is the trolley problem.

Lesson by Eleanor Nelsen, animation by Eoin Duffy.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:56

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions