Return to Video

What ants teach us about the brain, cancer and the Internet

  • 0:01 - 0:03
    I study ants
  • 0:03 - 0:06
    in the desert, in the tropical forest
  • 0:06 - 0:08
    and in my kitchen,
  • 0:08 - 0:12
    and in the hills around Silicon Valley where I live.
  • 0:12 - 0:13
    I've recently realized that ants
  • 0:13 - 0:16
    are using interactions differently
  • 0:16 - 0:17
    in different environments,
  • 0:17 - 0:19
    and that got me thinking
    that we could learn from this
  • 0:19 - 0:21
    about other systems,
  • 0:21 - 0:26
    like brains and data networks that we engineer,
  • 0:26 - 0:29
    and even cancer.
  • 0:29 - 0:31
    So what all these systems have in common
  • 0:31 - 0:34
    is that there's no central control.
  • 0:34 - 0:38
    An ant colony consists of sterile female workers --
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    those are the ants you see walking around —
  • 0:40 - 0:42
    and then one or more reproductive females
  • 0:42 - 0:44
    who just lay the eggs.
  • 0:44 - 0:46
    They don't give any instructions.
  • 0:46 - 0:48
    Even though they're called queens,
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    they don't tell anybody what to do.
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    So in an ant colony, there's no one in charge,
  • 0:54 - 0:57
    and all systems like this without central control
  • 0:57 - 1:01
    are regulated using very simple interactions.
  • 1:01 - 1:03
    Ants interact using smell.
  • 1:03 - 1:05
    They smell with their antennae,
  • 1:05 - 1:08
    and they interact with their antennae,
  • 1:08 - 1:11
    so when one ant touches another with its antennae,
  • 1:11 - 1:13
    it can tell, for example, if the other ant
  • 1:13 - 1:14
    is a nestmate
  • 1:14 - 1:19
    and what task that other ant has been doing.
  • 1:19 - 1:22
    So here you see a lot of ants moving around
  • 1:22 - 1:24
    and interacting in a lab arena
  • 1:24 - 1:27
    that's connected by tubes to two other arenas.
  • 1:27 - 1:30
    So when one ant meets another,
  • 1:30 - 1:32
    it doesn't matter which ant it meets,
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    and they're actually not transmitting
  • 1:34 - 1:37
    any kind of complicated signal or message.
  • 1:37 - 1:39
    All that matters to the ant is the rate
  • 1:39 - 1:42
    at which it meets other ants.
  • 1:42 - 1:45
    And all of these interactions, taken together,
  • 1:45 - 1:47
    produce a network.
  • 1:47 - 1:50
    So this is the network of the ants
  • 1:50 - 1:52
    that you just saw moving around in the arena,
  • 1:52 - 1:56
    and it's this constantly shifting network
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    that produces the behavior of the colony,
  • 1:58 - 2:01
    like whether all the ants are hiding inside the nest,
  • 2:01 - 2:04
    or how many are going out to forage.
  • 2:04 - 2:05
    A brain actually works in the same way,
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    but what's great about ants is
  • 2:07 - 2:12
    that you can see the whole network as it happens.
  • 2:12 - 2:15
    There are more than 12,000 species of ants,
  • 2:15 - 2:17
    in every conceivable environment,
  • 2:17 - 2:20
    and they're using interactions differently
  • 2:20 - 2:22
    to meet different environmental challenges.
  • 2:22 - 2:25
    So one important environmental challenge
  • 2:25 - 2:27
    that every system has to deal with
  • 2:27 - 2:29
    is operating costs, just what it takes
  • 2:29 - 2:31
    to run the system.
  • 2:31 - 2:33
    And another environmental challenge is resources,
  • 2:33 - 2:36
    finding them and collecting them.
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    In the desert, operating costs are high
  • 2:39 - 2:40
    because water is scarce,
  • 2:40 - 2:43
    and the seed-eating ants that I study in the desert
  • 2:43 - 2:46
    have to spend water to get water.
  • 2:46 - 2:48
    So an ant outside foraging,
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    searching for seeds in the hot sun,
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    just loses water into the air.
  • 2:52 - 2:54
    But the colony gets its water
  • 2:54 - 2:55
    by metabolizing the fats out of the seeds
  • 2:55 - 2:57
    that they eat.
  • 2:57 - 3:00
    So in this environment, interactions are used
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    to activate foraging.
  • 3:02 - 3:04
    An outgoing forager doesn't go out unless
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    it gets enough interactions with returning foragers,
  • 3:07 - 3:09
    and what you see are the returning foragers
  • 3:09 - 3:11
    going into the tunnel, into the nest,
  • 3:11 - 3:13
    and meeting outgoing foragers on their way out.
  • 3:13 - 3:15
    This makes sense for the ant colony,
  • 3:15 - 3:17
    because the more food there is out there,
  • 3:17 - 3:19
    the more quickly the foragers find it,
  • 3:19 - 3:20
    the faster they come back,
  • 3:20 - 3:23
    and the more foragers they send out.
  • 3:23 - 3:26
    The system works to stay stopped,
  • 3:26 - 3:28
    unless something positive happens.
  • 3:28 - 3:32
    So interactions function to activate foragers.
  • 3:32 - 3:34
    And we've been studying
    the evolution of this system.
  • 3:34 - 3:36
    First of all, there's variation.
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    It turns out that colonies are different.
  • 3:38 - 3:41
    On dry days, some colonies forage less,
  • 3:41 - 3:42
    so colonies are different in how
  • 3:42 - 3:44
    they manage this trade-off
  • 3:44 - 3:47
    between spending water to search for seeds
  • 3:47 - 3:50
    and getting water back in the form of seeds.
  • 3:50 - 3:52
    And we're trying to understand why
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    some colonies forage less than others
  • 3:54 - 3:56
    by thinking about ants as neurons,
  • 3:56 - 3:59
    using models from neuroscience.
  • 3:59 - 4:01
    So just as a neuron adds up its stimulation
  • 4:01 - 4:03
    from other neurons to decide whether to fire,
  • 4:03 - 4:06
    an ant adds up its stimulation from other ants
  • 4:06 - 4:08
    to decide whether to forage.
  • 4:08 - 4:10
    And what we're looking for is whether there might be
  • 4:10 - 4:12
    small differences among colonies
  • 4:12 - 4:15
    in how many interactions each ant needs
  • 4:15 - 4:17
    before it's willing to go out and forage,
  • 4:17 - 4:21
    because a colony like that would forage less.
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    And this raises an analogous question about brains.
  • 4:24 - 4:25
    We talk about the brain,
  • 4:25 - 4:28
    but of course every brain is slightly different,
  • 4:28 - 4:30
    and maybe there are some individuals
  • 4:30 - 4:31
    or some conditions
  • 4:31 - 4:34
    in which the electrical properties of neurons are such
  • 4:34 - 4:38
    that they require more stimulus to fire,
  • 4:38 - 4:42
    and that would lead to differences in brain function.
  • 4:42 - 4:44
    So in order to ask evolutionary questions,
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    we need to know about reproductive success.
  • 4:47 - 4:49
    This is a map of the study site
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    where I have been tracking this population
  • 4:52 - 4:55
    of harvester ant colonies for 28 years,
  • 4:55 - 4:57
    which is about as long as a colony lives.
  • 4:57 - 4:59
    Each symbol is a colony,
  • 4:59 - 5:03
    and the size of the symbol is
    how many offspring it had,
  • 5:03 - 5:05
    because we were able to use genetic variation
  • 5:05 - 5:07
    to match up parent and offspring colonies,
  • 5:07 - 5:11
    that is, to figure out which colonies
  • 5:11 - 5:12
    were founded by a daughter queen
  • 5:12 - 5:15
    produced by which parent colony.
  • 5:15 - 5:17
    And this was amazing for me, after all these years,
  • 5:17 - 5:20
    to find out, for example, that colony 154,
  • 5:20 - 5:22
    whom I've known well for many years,
  • 5:22 - 5:24
    is a great-grandmother.
  • 5:24 - 5:25
    Here's her daughter colony,
  • 5:25 - 5:28
    here's her granddaughter colony,
  • 5:28 - 5:30
    and these are her great-granddaughter colonies.
  • 5:30 - 5:32
    And by doing this, I was able to learn
  • 5:32 - 5:36
    that offspring colonies resemble parent colonies
  • 5:36 - 5:38
    in their decisions about which days are so hot
  • 5:38 - 5:40
    that they don't forage,
  • 5:40 - 5:41
    and the offspring of parent colonies
  • 5:41 - 5:44
    live so far from each other that the ants never meet,
  • 5:44 - 5:46
    so the ants of the offspring colony
  • 5:46 - 5:49
    can't be learning this from the parent colony.
  • 5:49 - 5:50
    And so our next step is to look
  • 5:50 - 5:55
    for the genetic variation
    underlying this resemblance.
  • 5:55 - 5:59
    So then I was able to ask, okay, who's doing better?
  • 5:59 - 6:01
    Over the time of the study,
  • 6:01 - 6:02
    and especially in the past 10 years,
  • 6:02 - 6:06
    there's been a very severe and deepening drought
  • 6:06 - 6:08
    in the Southwestern U.S.,
  • 6:08 - 6:11
    and it turns out that the
    colonies that conserve water,
  • 6:11 - 6:15
    that stay in when it's really hot outside,
  • 6:15 - 6:18
    and thus sacrifice getting as much food as possible,
  • 6:18 - 6:21
    are the ones more likely to have offspring colonies.
  • 6:21 - 6:23
    So all this time, I thought that colony 154
  • 6:23 - 6:26
    was a loser, because on really dry days,
  • 6:26 - 6:28
    there'd be just this trickle of foraging,
  • 6:28 - 6:29
    while the other colonies were out
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    foraging, getting lots of food,
  • 6:31 - 6:34
    but in fact, colony 154 is a huge success.
  • 6:34 - 6:36
    She's a matriarch.
  • 6:36 - 6:39
    She's one of the rare great-grandmothers on the site.
  • 6:39 - 6:42
    To my knowledge, this is the first time
  • 6:42 - 6:43
    that we've been able to track
  • 6:43 - 6:46
    the ongoing evolution of collective behavior
  • 6:46 - 6:48
    in a natural population of animals
  • 6:48 - 6:53
    and find out what's actually working best.
  • 6:53 - 6:55
    Now, the Internet uses an algorithm
  • 6:55 - 6:58
    to regulate the flow of data
  • 6:58 - 7:00
    that's very similar to the one
  • 7:00 - 7:03
    that the harvester ants are using to regulate
  • 7:03 - 7:04
    the flow of foragers.
  • 7:04 - 7:08
    And guess what we call this analogy?
  • 7:08 - 7:09
    The anternet is coming.
  • 7:09 - 7:11
    (Applause)
  • 7:11 - 7:14
    So data doesn't leave the source computer
  • 7:14 - 7:17
    unless it gets a signal that there's enough bandwidth
  • 7:17 - 7:20
    for it to travel on.
  • 7:20 - 7:21
    In the early days of the Internet,
  • 7:21 - 7:24
    when operating costs were really high
  • 7:24 - 7:27
    and it was really important not to lose any data,
  • 7:27 - 7:29
    then the system was set up for interactions
  • 7:29 - 7:32
    to activate the flow of data.
  • 7:32 - 7:35
    It's interesting that the ants are using an algorithm
  • 7:35 - 7:38
    that's so similar to the one that we recently invented,
  • 7:38 - 7:41
    but this is only one of a handful of ant algorithms
  • 7:41 - 7:43
    that we know about,
  • 7:43 - 7:46
    and ants have had 130 million years
  • 7:46 - 7:48
    to evolve a lot of good ones,
  • 7:48 - 7:50
    and I think it's very likely
  • 7:50 - 7:52
    that some of the other 12,000 species
  • 7:52 - 7:55
    are going to have interesting algorithms
  • 7:55 - 7:56
    for data networks
  • 7:56 - 7:59
    that we haven't even thought of yet.
  • 7:59 - 8:02
    So what happens when operating costs are low?
  • 8:02 - 8:03
    Operating costs are low in the tropics,
  • 8:03 - 8:06
    because it's very humid, and it's easy for the ants
  • 8:06 - 8:08
    to be outside walking around.
  • 8:08 - 8:10
    But the ants are so abundant
  • 8:10 - 8:12
    and diverse in the tropics
  • 8:12 - 8:14
    that there's a lot of competition.
  • 8:14 - 8:16
    Whatever resource one species is using,
  • 8:16 - 8:20
    another species is likely to be using that
  • 8:20 - 8:22
    at the same time.
  • 8:22 - 8:25
    So in this environment, interactions are used
  • 8:25 - 8:26
    in the opposite way.
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    The system keeps going
  • 8:28 - 8:29
    unless something negative happens,
  • 8:29 - 8:32
    and one species that I study makes circuits
  • 8:32 - 8:34
    in the trees of foraging ants
  • 8:34 - 8:37
    going from the nest to a food source and back,
  • 8:37 - 8:38
    just round and round,
  • 8:38 - 8:40
    unless something negative happens,
  • 8:40 - 8:41
    like an interaction
  • 8:41 - 8:44
    with ants of another species.
  • 8:44 - 8:47
    So here's an example of ant security.
  • 8:47 - 8:49
    In the middle, there's an ant
  • 8:49 - 8:51
    plugging the nest entrance with its head
  • 8:51 - 8:54
    in response to interactions with another species.
  • 8:54 - 8:56
    Those are the little ones running around
  • 8:56 - 8:59
    with their abdomens up in the air.
  • 8:59 - 9:01
    But as soon as the threat is passed,
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    the entrance is open again,
  • 9:03 - 9:05
    and maybe there are situations
  • 9:05 - 9:06
    in computer security
  • 9:06 - 9:08
    where operating costs are low enough
  • 9:08 - 9:12
    that we could just block access temporarily
  • 9:12 - 9:14
    in response to an immediate threat,
  • 9:14 - 9:16
    and then open it again,
  • 9:16 - 9:17
    instead of trying to build
  • 9:17 - 9:21
    a permanent firewall or fortress.
  • 9:21 - 9:23
    So another environmental challenge
  • 9:23 - 9:25
    that all systems have to deal with
  • 9:25 - 9:30
    is resources, finding and collecting them.
  • 9:30 - 9:32
    And to do this, ants solve the problem
  • 9:32 - 9:33
    of collective search,
  • 9:33 - 9:35
    and this is a problem that's of great interest
  • 9:35 - 9:36
    right now in robotics,
  • 9:36 - 9:38
    because we've understood that,
  • 9:38 - 9:40
    rather than sending a single,
  • 9:40 - 9:43
    sophisticated, expensive robot out
  • 9:43 - 9:45
    to explore another planet
  • 9:45 - 9:47
    or to search a burning building,
  • 9:47 - 9:50
    that instead, it may be more effective
  • 9:50 - 9:54
    to get a group of cheaper robots
  • 9:54 - 9:57
    exchanging only minimal information,
  • 9:57 - 9:59
    and that's the way that ants do it.
  • 9:59 - 10:01
    So the invasive Argentine ant
  • 10:01 - 10:04
    makes expandable search networks.
  • 10:04 - 10:06
    They're good at dealing with the main problem
  • 10:06 - 10:07
    of collective search,
  • 10:07 - 10:10
    which is the trade-off between
  • 10:10 - 10:11
    searching very thoroughly
  • 10:11 - 10:13
    and covering a lot of ground.
  • 10:13 - 10:14
    And what they do is,
  • 10:14 - 10:16
    when there are many ants in a small space,
  • 10:16 - 10:19
    then each one can search very thoroughly
  • 10:19 - 10:20
    because there will be another ant nearby
  • 10:20 - 10:22
    searching over there,
  • 10:22 - 10:23
    but when there are a few ants
  • 10:23 - 10:25
    in a large space,
  • 10:25 - 10:28
    then they need to stretch out their paths
  • 10:28 - 10:29
    to cover more ground.
  • 10:29 - 10:32
    I think they use interactions to assess density,
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    so when they're really crowded,
  • 10:34 - 10:35
    they meet more often,
  • 10:35 - 10:37
    and they search more thoroughly.
  • 10:37 - 10:41
    Different ant species must use different algorithms,
  • 10:41 - 10:43
    because they've evolved to deal with
  • 10:43 - 10:45
    different resources,
  • 10:45 - 10:47
    and it could be really useful to know about this,
  • 10:47 - 10:49
    and so we recently asked ants
  • 10:49 - 10:51
    to solve the collective search problem
  • 10:51 - 10:53
    in the extreme environment
  • 10:53 - 10:54
    of microgravity
  • 10:54 - 10:56
    in the International Space Station.
  • 10:56 - 10:58
    When I first saw this picture, I thought,
  • 10:58 - 11:01
    Oh no, they've mounted the habitat vertically,
  • 11:01 - 11:03
    but then I realized that, of course, it doesn't matter.
  • 11:03 - 11:06
    So the idea here is that the ants
  • 11:06 - 11:08
    are working so hard to hang on
  • 11:08 - 11:11
    to the wall or the floor or whatever you call it
  • 11:11 - 11:14
    that they're less likely to interact,
  • 11:14 - 11:15
    and so the relationship between
  • 11:15 - 11:17
    how crowded they are and how often they meet
  • 11:17 - 11:19
    would be messed up.
  • 11:19 - 11:21
    We're still analyzing the data.
  • 11:21 - 11:22
    I don't have the results yet.
  • 11:22 - 11:24
    But it would be interesting to know
  • 11:24 - 11:27
    how other species solve this problem
  • 11:27 - 11:29
    in different environments on Earth,
  • 11:29 - 11:30
    and so we're setting up a program
  • 11:30 - 11:33
    to encourage kids around the world
  • 11:33 - 11:35
    to try this experiment with different species.
  • 11:35 - 11:37
    It's very simple.
  • 11:37 - 11:39
    It can be done with cheap materials.
  • 11:39 - 11:42
    And that way, we could make a global map
  • 11:42 - 11:45
    of ant collective search algorithms.
  • 11:45 - 11:48
    And I think it's pretty likely that the invasive species,
  • 11:48 - 11:50
    the ones that come into our buildings,
  • 11:50 - 11:52
    are going to be really good at this,
  • 11:52 - 11:53
    because they're in your kitchen
  • 11:53 - 11:57
    because they're really good
    at finding food and water.
  • 11:57 - 12:01
    So the most familiar resource for ants
  • 12:01 - 12:02
    is a picnic,
  • 12:02 - 12:04
    and this is a clustered resource.
  • 12:04 - 12:05
    When there's one piece of fruit,
  • 12:05 - 12:08
    there's likely to be another piece of fruit nearby,
  • 12:08 - 12:11
    and the ants that specialize on clustered resources
  • 12:11 - 12:13
    use interactions for recruitment.
  • 12:13 - 12:14
    So when one ant meets another,
  • 12:14 - 12:16
    or when it meets a chemical deposited
  • 12:16 - 12:18
    on the ground by another,
  • 12:18 - 12:19
    then it changes direction to follow
  • 12:19 - 12:21
    in the direction of the interaction,
  • 12:21 - 12:23
    and that's how you get the trail of ants
  • 12:23 - 12:24
    sharing your picnic.
  • 12:24 - 12:26
    Now this is a place where I think we might be able
  • 12:26 - 12:30
    to learn something from ants about cancer.
  • 12:30 - 12:32
    I mean, first, it's obvious that we could do a lot
  • 12:32 - 12:33
    to prevent cancer
  • 12:33 - 12:36
    by not allowing people to spread around
  • 12:36 - 12:38
    or sell the toxins that promote
  • 12:38 - 12:41
    the evolution of cancer in our bodies,
  • 12:41 - 12:43
    but I don't think the ants can help us much with this
  • 12:43 - 12:46
    because ants never poison their own colonies.
  • 12:46 - 12:48
    But we might be able to learn something from ants
  • 12:48 - 12:50
    about treating cancer.
  • 12:50 - 12:52
    There are many different kinds of cancer.
  • 12:52 - 12:55
    Each one originates in a particular part of the body,
  • 12:55 - 12:58
    and then some kinds of cancer will spread
  • 12:58 - 13:01
    or metastasize to particular other tissues
  • 13:01 - 13:03
    where they must be getting
    resources that they need.
  • 13:03 - 13:05
    So if you think from the perspective
  • 13:05 - 13:07
    of early metastatic cancer cells
  • 13:07 - 13:09
    as they're out searching around
  • 13:09 - 13:11
    for the resources that they need,
  • 13:11 - 13:13
    if those resources are clustered,
  • 13:13 - 13:16
    they're likely to use interactions for recruitment,
  • 13:16 - 13:19
    and if we can figure out how
    cancer cells are recruiting,
  • 13:19 - 13:22
    then maybe we could set traps
  • 13:22 - 13:26
    to catch them before they become established.
  • 13:26 - 13:29
    So ants are using interactions in different ways
  • 13:29 - 13:31
    in a huge variety of environments,
  • 13:31 - 13:33
    and we could learn from this
  • 13:33 - 13:35
    about other systems that operate
  • 13:35 - 13:37
    without central control.
  • 13:37 - 13:39
    Using only simple interactions,
  • 13:39 - 13:41
    ant colonies have been performing
  • 13:41 - 13:45
    amazing feats for more than 130 million years.
  • 13:45 - 13:47
    We have a lot to learn from them.
  • 13:47 - 13:50
    Thank you.
  • 13:50 - 13:52
    (Applause)
Title:
What ants teach us about the brain, cancer and the Internet
Speaker:
Deborah Gordon
Description:

Ecologist Deborah Gordon studies ants wherever she can find them -- in the desert, in the tropics, in her kitchen ... In this fascinating talk, she explains her obsession with insects most of us would happily swat away without a second thought. She argues that ant life provides a useful model for learning about many other topics, including disease, technology and the human brain.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
14:09

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions