Return to Video

Why do we make irrational decisions? - Sara Garofalo

  • 0:07 - 0:09
    Let's say you're on a game show.
  • 0:09 - 0:12
    You've already earned $1000
    in the first round
  • 0:12 - 0:15
    when you land on the bonus space.
  • 0:15 - 0:17
    Now, you have a choice.
  • 0:17 - 0:20
    You can either take
    a $500 bonus guaranteed
  • 0:20 - 0:23
    or you can flip a coin.
  • 0:23 - 0:26
    If it's heads, you win $1000 bonus.
  • 0:26 - 0:29
    If it's tails, you get no bonus at all.
  • 0:29 - 0:34
    In the second round, you've earned $2000
    when you land on the penalty space.
  • 0:34 - 0:36
    Now you have another choice.
  • 0:36 - 0:39
    You can either take a $500 loss,
  • 0:39 - 0:42
    or try your luck at the coin flip.
  • 0:42 - 0:44
    If it's heads, you lose nothing,
  • 0:44 - 0:49
    but if it's tails, you lose $1000 instead.
  • 0:49 - 0:50
    If you're like most people,
  • 0:50 - 0:54
    you probably chose to take
    the guaranteed bonus in the first round
  • 0:54 - 0:57
    and flip the coin in the second round.
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    But if you think about it,
    this makes no sense.
  • 1:00 - 1:04
    The odds and outcomes in both rounds
    are exactly the same.
  • 1:04 - 1:09
    So why does the second round
    seem much scarier?
  • 1:09 - 1:13
    The answer lies in a phenomenon
    known as loss aversion.
  • 1:13 - 1:15
    Under rational economic theory,
  • 1:15 - 1:19
    our decisions should follow a simple
    mathematical equation
  • 1:19 - 1:23
    that weighs the level of risk
    against the amount at stake.
  • 1:23 - 1:25
    But studies have found
    that for many people,
  • 1:25 - 1:29
    the negative psychological impact
    we feel from losing something
  • 1:29 - 1:35
    is about twice as strong as the positive
    impact of gaining the same thing.
  • 1:35 - 1:40
    Loss aversion is one cognitive bias
    that arises from heuristics,
  • 1:40 - 1:44
    problem-solving approaches based on
    previous experience and intuition
  • 1:44 - 1:47
    rather than careful analysis.
  • 1:47 - 1:50
    And these mental shortcuts can lead
    to irrational decisions,
  • 1:50 - 1:51
    not like falling in love
  • 1:51 - 1:53
    or bungee jumping off a cliff,
  • 1:53 - 1:58
    but logical fallacies that can easily
    be proven wrong.
  • 1:58 - 2:04
    Situations involving probability are
    notoriously bad for applying heuristics.
  • 2:04 - 2:09
    For instance, say you were to roll a die
    with four green faces and two red faces
  • 2:09 - 2:11
    twenty times.
  • 2:11 - 2:14
    You can choose one of
    the following sequences of rolls,
  • 2:14 - 2:17
    and if it shows up,
    you'll win $25.
  • 2:17 - 2:19
    Which would you pick?
  • 2:19 - 2:24
    In one study, 65% of the participants
    who were all college students
  • 2:24 - 2:26
    chose sequence B
  • 2:26 - 2:30
    even though A is shorter
    and contained within B,
  • 2:30 - 2:32
    in other words, more likely.
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    This is what's called
    a conjunction fallacy.
  • 2:35 - 2:37
    Here, we expect to see more green rolls,
  • 2:37 - 2:42
    so our brains can trick us into picking
    the less likely option.
  • 2:42 - 2:46
    Heuristics are also terrible
    at dealing with numbers in general.
  • 2:46 - 2:49
    In one example, students were split
    into two groups.
  • 2:49 - 2:55
    The first group was asked whether
    Mahatma Gandhi died before or after age 9,
  • 2:55 - 3:00
    while the second was asked whether
    he died before or after age 140.
  • 3:00 - 3:03
    Both numbers were obviously way off,
  • 3:03 - 3:07
    but when the students were then asked
    to guess the actual age at which he died,
  • 3:07 - 3:10
    the first group's answers averaged to 50
  • 3:10 - 3:14
    while the second group's averaged to 67.
  • 3:14 - 3:17
    Even though the clearly wrong information
    in the initial questions
  • 3:17 - 3:19
    should have been irrelevant,
  • 3:19 - 3:22
    it still affected the students' estimates.
  • 3:22 - 3:25
    This is an example
    of the anchoring effect,
  • 3:25 - 3:28
    and it's often used in marketing
    and negotiations
  • 3:28 - 3:31
    to raise the prices
    that people are willing to pay.
  • 3:31 - 3:35
    So, if heuristics lead to
    all these wrong decisions,
  • 3:35 - 3:37
    why do we even have them?
  • 3:37 - 3:40
    Well, because they can be quite effective.
  • 3:40 - 3:41
    For most of human history,
  • 3:41 - 3:46
    survival depended on making quick
    decisions with limited information.
  • 3:46 - 3:50
    When there's no time to logically
    analyze all the possibilities,
  • 3:50 - 3:53
    heuristics can sometimes save our lives.
  • 3:53 - 3:57
    But today's environment requires
    far more complex decision-making,
  • 3:57 - 4:01
    and these decisions are more biased
    by unconscious factors than we think,
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    affecting everything from health
    and education
  • 4:04 - 4:06
    to finance and criminal justice.
  • 4:06 - 4:08
    We can't just shut off
    our brain's heuristics,
  • 4:08 - 4:11
    but we can learn to be aware of them.
  • 4:11 - 4:14
    When you come to
    a situation involving numbers,
  • 4:14 - 4:15
    probability,
  • 4:15 - 4:17
    or multiple details,
  • 4:17 - 4:18
    pause for a second
  • 4:18 - 4:23
    and consider that the intuitive answer
    might not be the right one after all.
Title:
Why do we make irrational decisions? - Sara Garofalo
Speaker:
Sara Garofalo
Description:

View full lesson: http://ed.ted.com/lessons/the-psychology-behind-irrational-decisions-sara-garofalo

Often people make decisions that are not “rational” from a purely economical point of view — meaning that they don’t necessarily lead to the best result. Why is that? Are we just bad at dealing with numbers and odds? Or is there a psychological mechanism behind it? Sara Garofalo explains heuristics, problem-solving approaches based on previous experience and intuition rather than analysis.

Lesson by Sara Garofalo, animation by TOGETHER.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:39

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions