Return to Video

Do we see reality as it is?

  • 0:01 - 0:03
    I love a great mystery,
  • 0:03 - 0:07
    and I'm fascinated by the greatest
    unsolved mystery in science,
  • 0:07 - 0:09
    perhaps because it's personal.
  • 0:10 - 0:12
    It's about who we are,
  • 0:12 - 0:14
    and I can't help but be curious.
  • 0:14 - 0:16
    The mystery is this:
  • 0:16 - 0:20
    What is the relationship
    between your brain
  • 0:20 - 0:21
    and your conscious experiences,
  • 0:21 - 0:24
    such as your experience
    of the taste of chocolate
  • 0:24 - 0:26
    or the feeling of velvet?
  • 0:27 - 0:28
    Now, this mystery is not new.
  • 0:29 - 0:33
    In 1868, Thomas Huxley wrote,
  • 0:33 - 0:38
    "How it is that anything so remarkable
    as a state of consciousness comes about
  • 0:38 - 0:41
    as the result of irritating nervous tissue
  • 0:41 - 0:43
    is just as unaccountable
  • 0:43 - 0:47
    as the appearance of the genie
    when Aladdin rubbed his lamp."
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    Now, Huxley knew that brain activity
  • 0:52 - 0:55
    and conscious experiences are correlated,
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    but he didn't know why.
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    To the science of his day,
    it was a mystery.
  • 1:00 - 1:02
    In the years since Huxley,
  • 1:02 - 1:06
    science has learned a lot
    about brain activity,
  • 1:06 - 1:08
    but the relationship
    between brain activity
  • 1:08 - 1:11
    and conscious experiences
    is still a mystery.
  • 1:11 - 1:15
    Why? Why have we made so little progress?
  • 1:15 - 1:19
    Well, some experts think
    that we can't solve this problem
  • 1:19 - 1:23
    because we lack the necessary
    concepts and intelligence.
  • 1:24 - 1:28
    We don't expect monkeys to solve
    problems in quantum mechanics,
  • 1:28 - 1:32
    and as it happens, we can't expect
    our species to solve this problem either.
  • 1:33 - 1:36
    Well, I disagree. I'm more optimistic.
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    I think we've simply
    made a false assumption.
  • 1:39 - 1:42
    Once we fix it, we just
    might solve this problem.
  • 1:42 - 1:45
    Today, I'd like tell you
    what that assumption is,
  • 1:45 - 1:47
    why it's false, and how to fix it.
  • 1:48 - 1:50
    Let's begin with a question:
  • 1:50 - 1:53
    Do we see reality as it is?
  • 1:53 - 1:55
    I open my eyes
  • 1:55 - 1:59
    and I have an experience that I describe
    as a red tomato a meter away.
  • 2:01 - 2:04
    As a result, I come to believe
    that in reality,
  • 2:04 - 2:06
    there's a red tomato a meter away.
  • 2:07 - 2:12
    I then close my eyes, and my experience
    changes to a gray field,
  • 2:12 - 2:18
    but is it still the case that in reality,
    there's a red tomato a meter away?
  • 2:18 - 2:22
    I think so, but could I be wrong?
  • 2:22 - 2:27
    Could I be misinterpreting
    the nature of my perceptions?
  • 2:27 - 2:31
    We have misinterpreted
    our perceptions before.
  • 2:31 - 2:34
    We used to think the Earth is flat,
    because it looks that way.
  • 2:35 - 2:38
    Pythagorus discovered that we were wrong.
  • 2:38 - 2:42
    Then we thought that the Earth
    is the unmoving center of the Universe,
  • 2:42 - 2:44
    again because it looks that way.
  • 2:44 - 2:49
    Copernicus and Galileo discovered,
    again, that we were wrong.
  • 2:49 - 2:53
    Galileo then wondered if we might
    be misinterpreting our experiences
  • 2:53 - 2:55
    in other ways.
  • 2:55 - 3:00
    He wrote: "I think that tastes,
    odors, colors, and so on
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    reside in consciousness.
  • 3:02 - 3:08
    Hence if the living creature were removed,
    all these qualities would be annihilated."
  • 3:09 - 3:11
    Now, that's a stunning claim.
  • 3:11 - 3:13
    Could Galileo be right?
  • 3:13 - 3:18
    Could we really be misinterpreting
    our experiences that badly?
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    What does modern science
    have to say about this?
  • 3:21 - 3:26
    Well, neuroscientists tell us
    that about a third of the brain's cortex
  • 3:26 - 3:28
    is engaged in vision.
  • 3:28 - 3:31
    When you simply open your eyes
    and look about this room,
  • 3:31 - 3:36
    billions of neurons
    and trillions of synapses are engaged.
  • 3:36 - 3:37
    Now, this is a bit surprising,
  • 3:37 - 3:40
    because to the extent that
    we think about vision at all,
  • 3:40 - 3:43
    we think of it as like a camera.
  • 3:43 - 3:47
    It just takes a picture
    of objective reality as it is.
  • 3:47 - 3:50
    Now, there is a part of vision
    that's like a camera:
  • 3:50 - 3:55
    the eye has a lens that focuses
    an image on the back of the eye
  • 3:55 - 3:58
    where there are 130 million
    photoreceptors,
  • 3:58 - 4:02
    so the eye is like a 130-megapixel camera.
  • 4:02 - 4:06
    But that doesn't explain
    the billions of neurons
  • 4:06 - 4:09
    and trillions of synapses
    that are engaged in vision.
  • 4:09 - 4:12
    What are these neurons up to?
  • 4:12 - 4:16
    Well, neuroscientists tell us
    that they are creating, in real time,
  • 4:16 - 4:20
    all the shapes, objects, colors,
    and motions that we see.
  • 4:20 - 4:24
    It feels like we're just taking a snapshot
    of this room the way it is,
  • 4:24 - 4:27
    but in fact, we're constructing
    everything that we see.
  • 4:27 - 4:30
    We don't construct
    the whole world at once.
  • 4:30 - 4:33
    We construct what we need in the moment.
  • 4:34 - 4:37
    Now, there are many demonstrations
    that are quite compelling
  • 4:37 - 4:39
    that we construct what we see.
  • 4:39 - 4:41
    I'll just show you two.
  • 4:41 - 4:47
    In this example, you see some red discs
    with bits cut out of them,
  • 4:47 - 4:49
    but if I just rotate
    the disks a little bit,
  • 4:49 - 4:54
    suddenly, you see a 3D cube
    pop out of the screen.
  • 4:54 - 4:57
    Now, the screen of course is flat,
  • 4:57 - 5:00
    so the three-dimensional cube
    that you're experiencing
  • 5:00 - 5:03
    must be your construction.
  • 5:03 - 5:05
    In this next example,
  • 5:05 - 5:10
    you see glowing blue bars
    with pretty sharp edges
  • 5:10 - 5:13
    moving across a field of dots.
  • 5:14 - 5:17
    In fact, no dots move.
  • 5:17 - 5:21
    All I'm doing from frame to frame
    is changing the colors of dots
  • 5:21 - 5:24
    from blue to black or black to blue.
  • 5:24 - 5:26
    But when I do this quickly,
  • 5:26 - 5:29
    your visual system creates
    the glowing blue bars
  • 5:29 - 5:32
    with the sharp edges and the motion.
  • 5:32 - 5:35
    There are many more examples,
    but these are just two
  • 5:35 - 5:38
    that you construct what you see.
  • 5:38 - 5:40
    But neuroscientists go further.
  • 5:41 - 5:46
    They say that we reconstruct reality.
  • 5:46 - 5:51
    So, when I have an experience
    that I describe as a red tomato,
  • 5:51 - 5:55
    that experience is actually
    an accurate reconstruction
  • 5:55 - 5:57
    of the properties of a real red tomato
  • 5:57 - 6:00
    that would exist
    even if I weren't looking.
  • 6:02 - 6:05
    Now, why would neuroscientists
    say that we don't just construct,
  • 6:05 - 6:07
    we reconstruct?
  • 6:07 - 6:09
    Well, the standard argument given
  • 6:09 - 6:12
    is usually an evolutionary one.
  • 6:13 - 6:15
    Those of our ancestors
    who saw more accurately
  • 6:15 - 6:20
    had a competitive advantage compared
    to those who saw less accurately,
  • 6:20 - 6:23
    and therefore they were more likely
    to pass on their genes.
  • 6:23 - 6:26
    We are the offspring of those
    who saw more accurately,
  • 6:26 - 6:29
    and so we can be confident that,
    in the normal case,
  • 6:29 - 6:32
    our perceptions are accurate.
  • 6:32 - 6:35
    You see this in the standard textbooks.
  • 6:35 - 6:37
    One textbook says, for example,
  • 6:37 - 6:39
    "Evolutionarily speaking,
  • 6:39 - 6:43
    vision is useful precisely
    because it is so accurate."
  • 6:43 - 6:48
    So the idea is that accurate perceptions
    are fitter perceptions.
  • 6:48 - 6:50
    They give you a survival advantage.
  • 6:50 - 6:52
    Now, is this correct?
  • 6:52 - 6:55
    Is this the right interpretation
    of evolutionary theory?
  • 6:55 - 6:58
    Well, let's first look at a couple
    of examples in nature.
  • 6:59 - 7:01
    The Australian jewel beetle
  • 7:01 - 7:04
    is dimpled, glossy and brown.
  • 7:04 - 7:07
    The female is flightless.
  • 7:07 - 7:11
    The male flies, looking,
    of course, for a hot female.
  • 7:11 - 7:15
    When he finds one, he alights and mates.
  • 7:15 - 7:17
    There's another species in the outback,
  • 7:17 - 7:18
    Homo sapiens.
  • 7:18 - 7:22
    The male of this species
    has a massive brain
  • 7:22 - 7:25
    that he uses to hunt for cold beer.
  • 7:26 - 7:27
    (Laughter)
  • 7:27 - 7:30
    And when he finds one, he drains it,
  • 7:30 - 7:33
    and sometimes throws the bottle
    into the outback.
  • 7:33 - 7:37
    Now, as it happens, these bottles
    are dimpled, glossy,
  • 7:37 - 7:41
    and just the right shade of brown
    to tickle the fancy of these beetles.
  • 7:43 - 7:46
    The males swarm all over
    the bottles trying to mate.
  • 7:48 - 7:50
    They lose all interest
    in the real females.
  • 7:50 - 7:55
    Classic case of the male
    leaving the female for the bottle.
  • 7:55 - 7:58
    (Laughter) (Applause)
  • 7:59 - 8:02
    The species almost went extinct.
  • 8:02 - 8:07
    Australia had to change its bottles
    to save its beetles.
  • 8:07 - 8:10
    (Laughter)
  • 8:10 - 8:14
    Now, the males had successfully
    found females for thousands,
  • 8:14 - 8:16
    perhaps millions of years.
  • 8:16 - 8:21
    It looked like they saw reality
    as it is, but apparently not.
  • 8:21 - 8:24
    Evolution had given them a hack.
  • 8:24 - 8:28
    A female is anything dimpled,
    glossy and brown,
  • 8:28 - 8:31
    the bigger the better.
  • 8:31 - 8:33
    (Laughter)
  • 8:33 - 8:37
    Even when crawling all over the bottle,
    the male couldn't discover his mistake.
  • 8:38 - 8:42
    Now, you might say, beetles, sure,
    they're very simple creatures,
  • 8:42 - 8:43
    but surely not mammals.
  • 8:43 - 8:46
    Mammals don't rely on tricks.
  • 8:46 - 8:52
    Well, I won't dwell on this,
    but you get the idea. (Laughter)
  • 8:52 - 8:55
    So this raises an important
    technical question:
  • 8:55 - 9:01
    Does natural selection really favor
    seeing reality as it is?
  • 9:02 - 9:05
    Fortunately, we don't have
    to wave our hands and guess;
  • 9:05 - 9:09
    evolution is a mathematically
    precise theory.
  • 9:09 - 9:12
    We can use the equations of evolution
    to check this out.
  • 9:12 - 9:16
    We can have various organisms
    in artificial worlds compete
  • 9:16 - 9:18
    and see which survive and which thrive,
  • 9:18 - 9:22
    which sensory systems are more fit.
  • 9:22 - 9:26
    A key notion in those
    equations is fitness.
  • 9:26 - 9:29
    Consider this steak:
  • 9:30 - 9:33
    What does this steak do
    for the fitness of an animal?
  • 9:33 - 9:39
    Well, for a hungry lion looking to eat,
    it enhances fitness.
  • 9:40 - 9:45
    For a well-fed lion looking to mate,
    it doesn't enhance fitness.
  • 9:46 - 9:50
    And for a rabbit in any state,
    it doesn't enhance fitness,
  • 9:50 - 9:54
    so fitness does depend
    on reality as it is, yes,
  • 9:54 - 9:58
    but also on the organism,
    its state and its action.
  • 9:58 - 10:02
    Fitness is not the same thing
    as reality as it is,
  • 10:02 - 10:05
    and it's fitness,
    and not reality as it is,
  • 10:05 - 10:09
    that figures centrally
    in the equations of evolution.
  • 10:09 - 10:13
    So, in my lab,
  • 10:13 - 10:16
    we have run hundreds of thousands
    of evolutionary game simulations
  • 10:16 - 10:19
    with lots of different
    randomly chosen worlds
  • 10:19 - 10:24
    and organisms that compete
    for resources in those worlds.
  • 10:24 - 10:28
    Some of the organisms
    see all of the reality,
  • 10:28 - 10:30
    others see just part of the reality,
  • 10:30 - 10:32
    and some see none of the reality,
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    only fitness.
  • 10:34 - 10:36
    Who wins?
  • 10:36 - 10:42
    Well, I hate to break it to you,
    but perception of reality goes extinct.
  • 10:42 - 10:44
    In almost every simulation,
  • 10:44 - 10:46
    organisms that see none of reality
  • 10:46 - 10:48
    but are just tuned to fitness
  • 10:48 - 10:54
    drive to extinction all the organisms
    that perceive reality as it is.
  • 10:54 - 10:58
    So the bottom line is, evolution
    does not favor veridical,
  • 10:58 - 11:00
    or accurate perceptions.
  • 11:00 - 11:04
    Those perceptions of reality go extinct.
  • 11:04 - 11:06
    Now, this is a bit stunning.
  • 11:06 - 11:09
    How can it be that not seeing
    the world accurately
  • 11:09 - 11:11
    gives us a survival advantage?
  • 11:11 - 11:13
    That is a bit counterintuitive.
  • 11:13 - 11:15
    But remember the jewel beetle.
  • 11:15 - 11:19
    The jewel beetle survived
    for thousands, perhaps millions of years,
  • 11:19 - 11:22
    using simple tricks and hacks.
  • 11:22 - 11:25
    What the equations
    of evolution are telling us
  • 11:25 - 11:30
    is that all organisms, including us,
    are in the same boat as the jewel beetle.
  • 11:30 - 11:32
    We do not see reality as it is.
  • 11:32 - 11:37
    We're shaped with tricks
    and hacks that keep us alive.
  • 11:37 - 11:39
    Still,
  • 11:39 - 11:41
    we need some help with our intuitions.
  • 11:41 - 11:45
    How can not perceiving
    reality as it is be useful?
  • 11:45 - 11:49
    Well, fortunately, we have
    a very helpful metaphor:
  • 11:49 - 11:52
    the desktop interface on your computer.
  • 11:52 - 11:56
    Consider that blue icon
    for a TED Talk that you're writing.
  • 11:56 - 12:00
    Now, the icon is blue and rectangular
  • 12:00 - 12:03
    and in the lower right corner
    of the desktop.
  • 12:03 - 12:08
    Does that mean that the text file itself
    in the computer is blue,
  • 12:08 - 12:12
    rectangular, and in the lower
    right-hand corner of the computer?
  • 12:12 - 12:13
    Of course not.
  • 12:13 - 12:18
    Anyone who thought that misinterprets
    the purpose of the interface.
  • 12:18 - 12:21
    It's not there to show you
    the reality of the computer.
  • 12:21 - 12:24
    In fact, it's there to hide that reality.
  • 12:24 - 12:26
    You don't want to know about the diodes
  • 12:26 - 12:28
    and resistors and all
    the megabytes of software.
  • 12:28 - 12:31
    If you had to deal with that,
    you could never write your text file
  • 12:31 - 12:32
    or edit your photo.
  • 12:32 - 12:37
    So the idea is that evolution
    has given us an interface
  • 12:37 - 12:41
    that hides reality and guides
    adaptive behavior.
  • 12:41 - 12:44
    Space and time, as you
    perceive them right now,
  • 12:44 - 12:47
    are your desktop.
  • 12:47 - 12:51
    Physical objects are simply icons
    in that desktop.
  • 12:52 - 12:54
    There's an obvious objection.
  • 12:54 - 12:58
    Hoffman, if you think that train
    coming down the track at 200 MPH
  • 12:58 - 13:01
    is just an icon of your desktop,
  • 13:01 - 13:03
    why don't you step in front of it?
  • 13:03 - 13:05
    And after you're gone,
    and your theory with you,
  • 13:05 - 13:09
    we'll know that there's more
    to that train than just an icon.
  • 13:09 - 13:11
    Well, I wouldn't step
    in front of that train
  • 13:11 - 13:12
    for the same reason
  • 13:12 - 13:16
    that I wouldn't carelessly drag
    that icon to the trash can:
  • 13:16 - 13:20
    not because I take the icon literally --
  • 13:20 - 13:23
    the file is not literally blue
    or rectangular --
  • 13:23 - 13:25
    but I do take it seriously.
  • 13:25 - 13:27
    I could lose weeks of work.
  • 13:27 - 13:30
    Similarly, evolution has shaped us
  • 13:30 - 13:34
    with perceptual symbols
    that are designed to keep us alive.
  • 13:35 - 13:37
    We'd better take them seriously.
  • 13:37 - 13:39
    If you see a snake, don't pick it up.
  • 13:40 - 13:43
    If you see a cliff, don't jump off.
  • 13:43 - 13:47
    They're designed to keep us safe,
    and we should take them seriously.
  • 13:47 - 13:49
    That does not mean that we
    should take them literally.
  • 13:49 - 13:52
    That's a logical error.
  • 13:52 - 13:55
    Another objection: There's
    nothing really new here.
  • 13:55 - 13:59
    Physicists have told us for a long time
    that the metal of that train looks solid
  • 13:59 - 14:03
    but really it's mostly empty space
    with microscopic particles zipping around.
  • 14:03 - 14:05
    There's nothing new here.
  • 14:05 - 14:07
    Well, not exactly.
  • 14:07 - 14:11
    It's like saying, I know that
    that blue icon on the desktop
  • 14:11 - 14:13
    is not the reality of the computer,
  • 14:13 - 14:17
    but if I pull out my trusty
    magnifying glass and look really closely,
  • 14:17 - 14:18
    I see little pixels,
  • 14:18 - 14:21
    and that's the reality of the computer.
  • 14:21 - 14:25
    Well, not really -- you're still
    on the desktop, and that's the point.
  • 14:25 - 14:28
    Those microscopic particles
    are still in space and time:
  • 14:28 - 14:30
    they're still in the user interface.
  • 14:30 - 14:34
    So I'm saying something far more radical
    than those physicists.
  • 14:35 - 14:36
    Finally, you might object,
  • 14:36 - 14:39
    look, we all see the train,
  • 14:39 - 14:42
    therefore none of us constructs the train.
  • 14:42 - 14:44
    But remember this example.
  • 14:44 - 14:47
    In this example, we all see a cube,
  • 14:48 - 14:50
    but the screen is flat,
  • 14:50 - 14:52
    so the cube that you see
    is the cube that you construct.
  • 14:54 - 14:56
    We all see a cube
  • 14:56 - 15:01
    because we all, each one of us,
    constructs the cube that we see.
  • 15:01 - 15:03
    The same is true of the train.
  • 15:03 - 15:07
    We all see a train because
    we each see the train that we construct,
  • 15:07 - 15:11
    and the same is true
    of all physical objects.
  • 15:12 - 15:17
    We're inclined to think that perception
    is like a window on reality as it is.
  • 15:17 - 15:22
    The theory of evolution is telling us
    that this is an incorrect interpretation
  • 15:22 - 15:24
    of our perceptions.
  • 15:25 - 15:29
    Instead, reality is more like a 3D desktop
  • 15:29 - 15:32
    that's designed to hide
    the complexity of the real world
  • 15:32 - 15:34
    and guide adaptive behavior.
  • 15:34 - 15:37
    Space as you perceive it is your desktop.
  • 15:37 - 15:40
    Physical objects are just
    the icons in that desktop.
  • 15:41 - 15:45
    We used to think that the Earth is flat
    because it looks that way.
  • 15:46 - 15:49
    Then we thought that the Earth
    is the unmoving center of reality
  • 15:49 - 15:50
    because it looks that way.
  • 15:50 - 15:52
    We were wrong.
  • 15:52 - 15:54
    We had misinterpreted our perceptions.
  • 15:55 - 15:58
    Now we believe that spacetime and objects
  • 15:58 - 16:01
    are the nature of reality as it is.
  • 16:01 - 16:05
    The theory of evolution is telling us
    that once again, we're wrong.
  • 16:05 - 16:10
    We're misinterpreting the content
    of our perceptual experiences.
  • 16:10 - 16:13
    There's something that exists
    when you don't look,
  • 16:13 - 16:16
    but it's not spacetime
    and physical objects.
  • 16:16 - 16:19
    It's as hard for us to let go
    of spacetime and objects
  • 16:19 - 16:23
    as it is for the jewel beetle
    to let go of its bottle.
  • 16:23 - 16:27
    Why? Because we're blind
    to our own blindnesses.
  • 16:28 - 16:31
    But we have an advantage
    over the jewel beetle:
  • 16:31 - 16:33
    our science and technology.
  • 16:33 - 16:35
    By peering through the lens of a telescope
  • 16:35 - 16:40
    we discovered that the Earth
    is not the unmoving center of reality,
  • 16:40 - 16:42
    and by peering through the lens
    of the theory of evolution
  • 16:42 - 16:45
    we discovered that spacetime and objects
  • 16:45 - 16:47
    are not the nature of reality.
  • 16:47 - 16:51
    When I have a perceptual experience
    that I describe as a red tomato,
  • 16:51 - 16:54
    I am interacting with reality,
  • 16:54 - 17:00
    but that reality is not a red tomato
    and is nothing like a red tomato.
  • 17:00 - 17:05
    Similarly, when I have an experience
    that I describe as a lion or a steak,
  • 17:05 - 17:07
    I'm interacting with reality,
  • 17:07 - 17:10
    but that reality is not a lion or a steak.
  • 17:10 - 17:12
    And here's the kicker:
  • 17:12 - 17:17
    When I have a perceptual experience
    that I describe as a brain, or neurons,
  • 17:17 - 17:19
    I am interacting with reality,
  • 17:19 - 17:22
    but that reality is not a brain or neurons
  • 17:22 - 17:26
    and is nothing like a brain or neurons.
  • 17:26 - 17:31
    And that reality, whatever it is,
  • 17:31 - 17:34
    is the real source of cause and effect
  • 17:34 - 17:38
    in the world -- not brains, not neurons.
  • 17:38 - 17:41
    Brains and neurons
    have no causal powers.
  • 17:41 - 17:43
    They cause none of our
    perceptual experiences,
  • 17:43 - 17:45
    and none of our behavior.
  • 17:45 - 17:51
    Brains and neurons are a species-specific
    set of symbols, a hack.
  • 17:51 - 17:53
    What does this mean
    for the mystery of consciousness?
  • 17:54 - 17:58
    Well, it opens up new possibilities.
  • 17:58 - 18:00
    For instance,
  • 18:00 - 18:07
    perhaps reality is some vast machine
    that causes our conscious experiences.
  • 18:07 - 18:10
    I doubt this, but it's worth exploring.
  • 18:10 - 18:16
    Perhaps reality is some vast,
    interacting network of conscious agents,
  • 18:16 - 18:21
    simple and complex, that cause
    each other's conscious experiences.
  • 18:21 - 18:24
    Actually, this isn't as crazy
    an idea as it seems,
  • 18:24 - 18:26
    and I'm currently exploring it.
  • 18:27 - 18:29
    But here's the point:
  • 18:29 - 18:32
    Once we let go of our massively intuitive
  • 18:32 - 18:36
    but massively false assumption
    about the nature of reality,
  • 18:36 - 18:40
    it opens up new ways to think
    about life's greatest mystery.
  • 18:41 - 18:46
    I bet that reality will end up
    turning out to be more fascinating
  • 18:46 - 18:50
    and unexpected than we've ever imagined.
  • 18:50 - 18:54
    The theory of evolution presents us
    with the ultimate dare:
  • 18:54 - 18:59
    Dare to recognize that perception
    is not about seeing truth,
  • 18:59 - 19:03
    it's about having kids.
  • 19:03 - 19:08
    And by the way, even this TED
    is just in your head.
  • 19:08 - 19:10
    Thank you very much.
  • 19:10 - 19:14
    (Applause)
  • 19:21 - 19:24
    Chris Anderson: If that's
    really you there, thank you.
  • 19:24 - 19:27
    So there's so much from this.
  • 19:27 - 19:30
    I mean, first of all, some people
    may just be profoundly depressed
  • 19:30 - 19:36
    at the thought that,
    if evolution does not favor reality,
  • 19:36 - 19:39
    I mean, doesn't that to some extent
    undermine all our endeavors here,
  • 19:39 - 19:42
    all our ability to think
    that we can think the truth,
  • 19:42 - 19:45
    possibly even including
    your own theory, if you go there?
  • 19:45 - 19:50
    Donald Hoffman: Well, this does not
    stop us from a successful science.
  • 19:50 - 19:53
    What we have is one theory
    that turned out to be false,
  • 19:53 - 19:57
    that perception is like reality
    and reality is like our perceptions.
  • 19:57 - 19:59
    That theory turns out to be false.
  • 19:59 - 20:00
    Okay, throw that theory away.
  • 20:00 - 20:04
    That doesn't stop us from now postulating
    all sorts of other theories
  • 20:04 - 20:05
    about the nature of reality,
  • 20:05 - 20:09
    so it's actually progress to recognize
    that one of our theories was false.
  • 20:09 - 20:11
    So science continues as normal.
    There's no problem here.
  • 20:11 - 20:14
    CA: So you think it's possible
    -- (Laughter) --
  • 20:14 - 20:18
    This is cool, but what you're saying
    I think is it's possible that evolution
  • 20:18 - 20:21
    can still get you to reason.
  • 20:21 - 20:23
    DH: Yes. Now that's a very,
    very good point.
  • 20:23 - 20:27
    The evolutionary game simulations that I
    showed were specifically about perception,
  • 20:27 - 20:30
    and they do show that our perceptions
    have been shaped
  • 20:30 - 20:32
    not to show us reality as it is,
  • 20:32 - 20:36
    but that does not mean the same thing
    about our logic or mathematics.
  • 20:36 - 20:40
    We haven't done these simulations,
    but my bet is that we'll find
  • 20:40 - 20:43
    that there are some selection pressures
    for our logic and our mathematics
  • 20:43 - 20:46
    to be at least in the direction of truth.
  • 20:46 - 20:48
    I mean, if you're like me,
    math and logic is not easy.
  • 20:48 - 20:52
    We don't get it all right, but at least
    the selection pressures are not
  • 20:52 - 20:54
    uniformly away from true math and logic.
  • 20:54 - 20:57
    So I think that we'll find that we have
    to look at each cognitive faculty
  • 20:57 - 21:00
    one at a time and see
    what evolution does to it.
  • 21:00 - 21:04
    What's true about perception may not
    be true about math and logic.
  • 21:04 - 21:08
    CA: I mean, really what you're proposing
    is a kind of modern-day Bishop Berkeley
  • 21:08 - 21:10
    interpretation of the world:
  • 21:10 - 21:13
    consciousness causes matter,
    not the other way around.
  • 21:13 - 21:15
    DH: Well, it's slightly
    different than Berkeley.
  • 21:15 - 21:19
    Berkeley thought that, he was a deist,
    and he thought that the ultimate
  • 21:19 - 21:21
    nature of reality is God
    and so forth,
  • 21:21 - 21:24
    and I don't need to go
    where Berkeley's going,
  • 21:24 - 21:27
    so it's quite a bit
    different from Berkeley.
  • 21:28 - 21:31
    I call this conscious realism.
    It's actually a very different approach.
  • 21:31 - 21:35
    CA: Don, I could literally talk with you
    for hours, and I hope to do that.
  • 21:35 - 21:37
    Thanks so much for that.
    DH: Thank you. (Applause)
Title:
Do we see reality as it is?
Speaker:
Donald Hoffman
Description:

Cognitive scientist Donald Hoffman is trying to answer a big question: Do we experience the world as it really is ... or as we need it to be? In this ever so slightly mind-blowing talk, he ponders how our minds construct reality for us.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
21:50
  • 2:35 - 2:38 Pythagorus discovered that we were wrong.

    It's "Pythagoras".

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions