1 00:00:07,257 --> 00:00:12,417 In 1861, two scientists got into a very brainy argument. 2 00:00:12,417 --> 00:00:16,447 Specifically, they had opposing ideas of how speech and memory 3 00:00:16,447 --> 00:00:18,923 operated within the human brain. 4 00:00:18,923 --> 00:00:21,540 Ernest Aubertin, with his localistic model, 5 00:00:21,540 --> 00:00:23,822 argued that a particular region or the brain 6 00:00:23,822 --> 00:00:26,654 was devoted to each separate process. 7 00:00:26,654 --> 00:00:30,744 Pierre Gratiolet, on the other hand, argued for the distributed model, 8 00:00:30,744 --> 00:00:32,739 where different regions work together 9 00:00:32,739 --> 00:00:35,507 to accomplish all of these various functions. 10 00:00:35,507 --> 00:00:39,105 The debate they began reverberated throughout the rest of the century, 11 00:00:39,105 --> 00:00:42,937 involving some of the greatest scientific minds of the time. 12 00:00:42,937 --> 00:00:46,955 Aubertin and his localistic model had some big names on his side. 13 00:00:46,955 --> 00:00:50,164 In the 17th century, René Descartes had assigned the quality 14 00:00:50,164 --> 00:00:54,729 of free will and the human soul to the pineal gland. 15 00:00:54,729 --> 00:00:58,899 And in the late 18th century, a young student named Franz Joseph Gall 16 00:00:58,899 --> 00:01:04,193 had observed that the best memorizers in his class had the most prominent eyes 17 00:01:04,193 --> 00:01:06,704 and decided that this was due to higher development 18 00:01:06,704 --> 00:01:09,140 in the adjacent part of the brain. 19 00:01:09,140 --> 00:01:12,968 As a physician, Gall went on to establish the study of phrenology, 20 00:01:12,968 --> 00:01:16,050 which held that strong mental faculties corresponded to 21 00:01:16,050 --> 00:01:20,843 highly developed brain regions, observable as bumps in the skull. 22 00:01:20,843 --> 00:01:24,787 The widespread popularity of phrenology throughout the early 19th century 23 00:01:24,787 --> 00:01:28,040 tipped the scales towards Aubertin's localism. 24 00:01:28,040 --> 00:01:31,699 But the problem was that Gall had never bothered to scientifically test 25 00:01:31,699 --> 00:01:34,800 whether the individual brain maps he had constructed 26 00:01:34,800 --> 00:01:36,930 applied to all people. 27 00:01:36,930 --> 00:01:40,341 And in the 1840's, Pierre Flourens challenged phrenology 28 00:01:40,341 --> 00:01:43,594 by selectively destroying parts of animal brains 29 00:01:43,594 --> 00:01:46,125 and observing which functions were lost. 30 00:01:46,125 --> 00:01:48,019 Flourens found that damaging the cortex 31 00:01:48,019 --> 00:01:51,323 interfered with judgement or movement in general, 32 00:01:51,323 --> 00:01:55,611 but failed to identify any region associated with one specific function, 33 00:01:55,611 --> 00:02:00,393 concluding that the cortex carried out brain functions as an entire unit. 34 00:02:00,393 --> 00:02:04,550 Flourens had scored a victory for Gratiolet, but it was not to last. 35 00:02:04,550 --> 00:02:07,258 Gall's former student, Jean-Baptiste Bouillaud, 36 00:02:07,258 --> 00:02:09,003 challenged Flourens' conclusion, 37 00:02:09,003 --> 00:02:11,470 observing that patients with speech disorders 38 00:02:11,470 --> 00:02:14,046 all had damage to the frontal lobe. 39 00:02:14,046 --> 00:02:18,734 And after Paul Broca's 1861 autopsy of a patient who had lost the power 40 00:02:18,734 --> 00:02:21,812 to produce speech, but not the power to understand it, 41 00:02:21,812 --> 00:02:24,749 revealed highly localized frontal lobe damage, 42 00:02:24,749 --> 00:02:27,551 the distributed model seemed doomed. 43 00:02:27,551 --> 00:02:29,158 Localism took off. 44 00:02:29,158 --> 00:02:33,260 In the 1870's, Karl Wernicke associated part of the left temporal lobe 45 00:02:33,260 --> 00:02:35,188 with speech comprehension. 46 00:02:35,188 --> 00:02:37,587 Soon after, Eduard Hitzig and Gustav Fritsch 47 00:02:37,587 --> 00:02:41,673 stimulated a dog's cortex and discovered a frontal lobe region 48 00:02:41,673 --> 00:02:44,281 responsible for muscular movements. 49 00:02:44,281 --> 00:02:47,632 Building on their work, David Ferrier mapped each piece of cortex 50 00:02:47,632 --> 00:02:50,741 associated with moving a part of the body. 51 00:02:50,741 --> 00:02:56,739 And in 1909, Korbinian Brodmann built his own cortex map with 52 separate areas. 52 00:02:56,739 --> 00:03:01,226 It appeared that the victory of Aubertin's localistic model was sealed. 53 00:03:01,226 --> 00:03:04,877 But neurologist Karl Wernicke had come up with an interesting idea. 54 00:03:04,877 --> 00:03:08,825 He reasoned that since the regions for speech production and comprehension 55 00:03:08,825 --> 00:03:10,373 were not adjacent, 56 00:03:10,373 --> 00:03:13,109 then injuring the area connecting them might result 57 00:03:13,109 --> 00:03:18,148 in a special type of language loss, now known as receptive aphasia. 58 00:03:18,148 --> 00:03:21,004 Wernicke's connectionist model helped explain disorders 59 00:03:21,004 --> 00:03:25,028 that didn't result from the dysfunction of just one area. 60 00:03:25,028 --> 00:03:28,046 Modern neuroscience tools reveal a brain more complex than 61 00:03:28,046 --> 00:03:32,043 Gratiolet, Aubertin, or even Wernicke imagined. 62 00:03:32,043 --> 00:03:36,239 Today, the hippocampus is associated with two distinct brain functions: 63 00:03:36,239 --> 00:03:40,776 creating memories and processing location in space. 64 00:03:40,776 --> 00:03:43,337 We also now measure two kinds of connectivity: 65 00:03:43,337 --> 00:03:46,122 anatomical connectivity between two adjoining 66 00:03:46,122 --> 00:03:48,175 regions of cortex working together, 67 00:03:48,175 --> 00:03:51,033 and functional connectivity between separated regions 68 00:03:51,033 --> 00:03:54,287 working together to accomplish one process. 69 00:03:54,287 --> 00:03:56,285 A seemingly basic function like vision 70 00:03:56,285 --> 00:03:59,229 is actually composed of many smaller functions, 71 00:03:59,229 --> 00:04:01,442 with different parts of the cortex representing 72 00:04:01,442 --> 00:04:04,581 shape, color and location in space. 73 00:04:04,581 --> 00:04:07,856 When certain areas stop functioning, we may recognize an object, 74 00:04:07,856 --> 00:04:10,536 but not see it, or vice versa. 75 00:04:10,536 --> 00:04:14,729 There are even different kinds of memory for facts and for routines. 76 00:04:14,729 --> 00:04:17,192 And remembering something like your first bicycle 77 00:04:17,192 --> 00:04:20,996 involves a network of different regions each representing the concept 78 00:04:20,996 --> 00:04:24,491 of vehicles, the bicycle's shape, the sound of the bell, 79 00:04:24,491 --> 00:04:27,422 and the emotions associated with that memory. 80 00:04:27,422 --> 00:04:31,360 In the end, both Gratiolet and Aubertin turned out to be right. 81 00:04:31,360 --> 00:04:35,147 And we still use both of their models to understand how cognition happens. 82 00:04:35,147 --> 00:04:39,515 For example, we can now measure brain activity on such a fine time scale 83 00:04:39,515 --> 00:04:42,971 that we can see the individual localized processes that comprise 84 00:04:42,971 --> 00:04:45,240 a single act of remembering. 85 00:04:45,240 --> 00:04:48,362 But it is the integration of these different processes and regions 86 00:04:48,362 --> 00:04:51,268 that creates the coherent memory we experience. 87 00:04:51,268 --> 00:04:55,328 The supposedly competing theories prove to be two aspects 88 00:04:55,328 --> 00:04:57,326 of a more comprehensive model, 89 00:04:57,326 --> 00:04:59,302 which will in turn be revised and refined 90 00:04:59,302 --> 00:05:04,060 as our scientific techologies and methods for understanding the brain improve.