WEBVTT 00:00:06.979 --> 00:00:11.559 To human eyes, the world at night is a formless canvas of grey. 00:00:11.559 --> 00:00:14.313 Many nocturnal animals, on the other hand, 00:00:14.313 --> 00:00:19.599 experience a rich and varied world bursting with details, shapes, and colors. 00:00:19.599 --> 00:00:23.490 What is it, then, that separates moths from men? 00:00:23.490 --> 00:00:26.398 Moths and many other nocturnal animals see at night 00:00:26.398 --> 00:00:30.148 because their eyes are adapted to compensate for the lack of light. 00:00:30.148 --> 00:00:32.909 All eyes, whether nocturnal or not, 00:00:32.909 --> 00:00:36.757 depend on photoreceptors in the retina to detect light particles, 00:00:36.757 --> 00:00:38.938 known as photons. 00:00:38.938 --> 00:00:42.969 Photoreceptors then report information about these photons to other cells 00:00:42.969 --> 00:00:44.869 in the retina and brain. 00:00:44.869 --> 00:00:48.429 The brain sifts through that information and uses it to build up an image 00:00:48.429 --> 00:00:50.691 of the environment the eye perceives. 00:00:50.691 --> 00:00:54.399 The brighter the light is, the more photons hit the eye. 00:00:54.399 --> 00:00:55.729 On a sunny day, 00:00:55.729 --> 00:01:00.259 upwards of 100 million times more photons are available to the eye 00:01:00.259 --> 00:01:02.650 than on a cloudy, moonless night. 00:01:02.650 --> 00:01:05.420 Photons aren't just less numerous in darkness, 00:01:05.420 --> 00:01:08.890 but they also hit the eye in a less reliable way. 00:01:08.890 --> 00:01:11.839 This means the information that photoreceptors collect 00:01:11.839 --> 00:01:13.450 will vary over time, 00:01:13.450 --> 00:01:15.600 as will the quality of the image. 00:01:15.600 --> 00:01:20.620 In darkness, trying to detect the sparse scattering of randomly arriving photons 00:01:20.620 --> 00:01:24.010 is too difficult for the eyes of most daytime animals. 00:01:24.010 --> 00:01:27.841 But for night creatures, it's just a matter of adaptation. 00:01:27.841 --> 00:01:31.391 One of these adaptations is size. 00:01:31.391 --> 00:01:35.980 Take the tarsier, whose eyeballs are each as big as its brain, 00:01:35.980 --> 00:01:39.990 giving it the biggest eyes compared to head size of all mammals. 00:01:39.990 --> 00:01:45.461 If humans had the same brain to eye ratio, our eyes would be the size of grapefruits. 00:01:45.461 --> 00:01:48.830 The tarsier's enlarged orbs haven't evolved to make it cuter, however, 00:01:48.830 --> 00:01:51.881 but to gather as much light as possible. 00:01:51.881 --> 00:01:55.041 Bigger eyes can have larger openings, called pupils, 00:01:55.041 --> 00:01:56.561 and larger lenses, 00:01:56.561 --> 00:01:59.831 allowing for more light to be focused on the receptors. 00:01:59.831 --> 00:02:04.223 While tarsiers scan the nocturnal scene with their enormous peepers, 00:02:04.223 --> 00:02:08.432 cats use gleaming eyes to do the same. 00:02:08.432 --> 00:02:12.352 Cats' eyes get their shine from a structure called the tapetum lucidum 00:02:12.352 --> 00:02:14.791 that sits behind the photoreceptors. 00:02:14.791 --> 00:02:18.733 This structure is made from layers of mirror-like cells containing crystals 00:02:18.733 --> 00:02:22.336 that send incoming light bouncing back towards the photoreceptors 00:02:22.336 --> 00:02:24.062 and out of the eye. 00:02:24.062 --> 00:02:25.812 This results in an eerie glow, 00:02:25.812 --> 00:02:30.342 and it also gives the photoreceptors a second chance to detect photons. 00:02:30.342 --> 00:02:35.973 In fact, this system has inspired the artificial cats' eyes we use on our roads. 00:02:35.973 --> 00:02:39.653 Toads, on the other hand, have adapted to take it slow. 00:02:39.653 --> 00:02:41.376 They can form an image 00:02:41.376 --> 00:02:45.701 even when just a single photon hits each photoreceptor per second. 00:02:45.701 --> 00:02:47.846 They accomplish this with photoreceptors 00:02:47.846 --> 00:02:51.353 that are more than 25 times slower than human ones. 00:02:51.353 --> 00:02:54.486 This means toads can collect photons for up to four seconds, 00:02:54.486 --> 00:02:57.362 allowing them to gather many more than our eyes do 00:02:57.362 --> 00:02:59.743 at each visual time interval. 00:02:59.743 --> 00:03:03.762 The downside is that this causes toads to react very slowly 00:03:03.762 --> 00:03:08.034 because they're only receiving an updated image every four seconds. 00:03:08.034 --> 00:03:11.474 Fortunately, they're accustomed to targeting sluggish prey. 00:03:11.474 --> 00:03:14.793 Meanwhile, the night is also buzzing with insects, 00:03:14.793 --> 00:03:16.792 such as hawk moths, 00:03:16.792 --> 00:03:21.254 which can see their favorite flowers in color, even on a starlit night. 00:03:21.254 --> 00:03:23.383 They achieve this by a surprising move - 00:03:23.383 --> 00:03:26.213 getting rid of details in their visual perception. 00:03:26.213 --> 00:03:29.754 Information from neighboring photoreceptors is grouped in their brains, 00:03:29.754 --> 00:03:32.244 so the photon catch of each group is higher 00:03:32.244 --> 00:03:34.745 compared to individual receptors. 00:03:34.745 --> 00:03:38.422 However, grouping photoreceptors loses details in the image, 00:03:38.422 --> 00:03:42.014 as fine details require a fine grid of photoreceptors, 00:03:42.014 --> 00:03:45.784 each detecting photons from one small point in space. 00:03:45.784 --> 00:03:49.574 The trick is to balance the need for photons with the loss of detail 00:03:49.574 --> 00:03:51.243 to still find their flowers. 00:03:51.243 --> 00:03:54.194 Whether eyes are slow, enormous, shiny, or coarse, 00:03:54.194 --> 00:03:57.245 it's the combination of these biological adaptations 00:03:57.245 --> 00:04:00.956 that gives nocturnal animals their unique visual powers. 00:04:00.956 --> 00:04:03.907 Imagine what it might be like to witness through their eyes 00:04:03.907 --> 00:04:06.676 the world that wakes up when the Sun goes down.