1 00:00:00,545 --> 00:00:04,077 I grew up watching Star Trek. I love Star Trek. 2 00:00:04,077 --> 00:00:08,539 Star Trek made me want to see alien creatures, 3 00:00:08,539 --> 00:00:10,842 creatures from a far-distant world. 4 00:00:10,842 --> 00:00:13,629 But basically, I figured out that I could find 5 00:00:13,629 --> 00:00:16,606 those alien creatures right on Earth. 6 00:00:16,606 --> 00:00:19,259 And what I do is I study insects. 7 00:00:19,259 --> 00:00:22,515 I'm obsessed with insects, particularly insect flight. 8 00:00:22,515 --> 00:00:25,656 I think the evolution of insect flight is perhaps 9 00:00:25,656 --> 00:00:28,398 one of the most important events in the history of life. 10 00:00:28,398 --> 00:00:30,635 Without insects, there'd be no flowering plants. 11 00:00:30,635 --> 00:00:32,551 Without flowering plants, there would be no 12 00:00:32,551 --> 00:00:35,688 clever, fruit-eating primates giving TED Talks. 13 00:00:35,688 --> 00:00:37,988 (Laughter) 14 00:00:37,988 --> 00:00:39,975 Now, 15 00:00:39,975 --> 00:00:43,014 David and Hidehiko and Ketaki 16 00:00:43,014 --> 00:00:46,459 gave a very compelling story about 17 00:00:46,459 --> 00:00:49,264 the similarities between fruit flies and humans, 18 00:00:49,264 --> 00:00:50,753 and there are many similarities, 19 00:00:50,753 --> 00:00:53,755 and so you might think that if humans are similar to fruit flies, 20 00:00:53,755 --> 00:00:57,552 the favorite behavior of a fruit fly might be this, for example -- 21 00:00:57,552 --> 00:00:59,834 (Laughter) 22 00:00:59,834 --> 00:01:03,025 but in my talk, I don't want to emphasize on the similarities 23 00:01:03,025 --> 00:01:06,092 between humans and fruit flies, but rather the differences, 24 00:01:06,092 --> 00:01:11,379 and focus on the behaviors that I think fruit flies excel at doing. 25 00:01:11,379 --> 00:01:14,235 And so I want to show you a high-speed video sequence 26 00:01:14,235 --> 00:01:18,170 of a fly shot at 7,000 frames per second in infrared lighting, 27 00:01:18,170 --> 00:01:22,380 and to the right, off-screen, is an electronic looming predator 28 00:01:22,380 --> 00:01:23,815 that is going to go at the fly. 29 00:01:23,815 --> 00:01:25,653 The fly is going to sense this predator. 30 00:01:25,653 --> 00:01:28,108 It is going to extend its legs out. 31 00:01:28,108 --> 00:01:29,721 It's going to sashay away 32 00:01:29,721 --> 00:01:32,286 to live to fly another day. 33 00:01:32,286 --> 00:01:34,648 Now I have carefully cropped this sequence 34 00:01:34,648 --> 00:01:37,808 to be exactly the duration of a human eye blink, 35 00:01:37,808 --> 00:01:40,642 so in the time that it would take you to blink your eye, 36 00:01:40,642 --> 00:01:43,907 the fly has seen this looming predator, 37 00:01:43,907 --> 00:01:50,075 estimated its position, initiated a motor pattern to fly it away, 38 00:01:50,075 --> 00:01:54,539 beating its wings at 220 times a second as it does so. 39 00:01:54,539 --> 00:01:56,512 I think this is a fascinating behavior 40 00:01:56,512 --> 00:02:00,433 that shows how fast the fly's brain can process information. 41 00:02:00,433 --> 00:02:03,275 Now, flight -- what does it take to fly? 42 00:02:03,275 --> 00:02:06,139 Well, in order to fly, just as in a human aircraft, 43 00:02:06,139 --> 00:02:08,874 you need wings that can generate sufficient aerodynamic forces, 44 00:02:08,874 --> 00:02:12,420 you need an engine sufficient to generate the power required for flight, 45 00:02:12,420 --> 00:02:14,129 and you need a controller, 46 00:02:14,129 --> 00:02:16,755 and in the first human aircraft, the controller was basically 47 00:02:16,755 --> 00:02:21,067 the brain of Orville and Wilbur sitting in the cockpit. 48 00:02:21,067 --> 00:02:23,820 Now, how does this compare to a fly? 49 00:02:23,820 --> 00:02:27,071 Well, I spent a lot of my early career trying to figure out 50 00:02:27,071 --> 00:02:31,407 how insect wings generate enough force to keep the flies in the air. 51 00:02:31,407 --> 00:02:33,017 And you might have heard how engineers proved 52 00:02:33,017 --> 00:02:35,651 that bumblebees couldn't fly. 53 00:02:35,651 --> 00:02:38,271 Well, the problem was in thinking that the insect wings 54 00:02:38,271 --> 00:02:41,390 function in the way that aircraft wings work. But they don't. 55 00:02:41,390 --> 00:02:44,244 And we tackle this problem by building giant, 56 00:02:44,244 --> 00:02:47,676 dynamically scaled model robot insects 57 00:02:47,676 --> 00:02:51,012 that would flap in giant pools of mineral oil 58 00:02:51,012 --> 00:02:53,286 where we could study the aerodynamic forces. 59 00:02:53,286 --> 00:02:55,444 And it turns out that the insects flap their wings 60 00:02:55,444 --> 00:02:58,036 in a very clever way, at a very high angle of attack 61 00:02:58,036 --> 00:03:01,157 that creates a structure at the leading edge of the wing, 62 00:03:01,157 --> 00:03:04,356 a little tornado-like structure called a leading edge vortex, 63 00:03:04,356 --> 00:03:07,310 and it's that vortex that actually enables the wings 64 00:03:07,310 --> 00:03:10,669 to make enough force for the animal to stay in the air. 65 00:03:10,669 --> 00:03:13,097 But the thing that's actually most -- so, what's fascinating 66 00:03:13,097 --> 00:03:16,072 is not so much that the wing has some interesting morphology. 67 00:03:16,072 --> 00:03:19,717 What's clever is the way the fly flaps it, 68 00:03:19,717 --> 00:03:22,853 which of course ultimately is controlled by the nervous system, 69 00:03:22,853 --> 00:03:25,500 and this is what enables flies to perform 70 00:03:25,500 --> 00:03:28,307 these remarkable aerial maneuvers. 71 00:03:28,307 --> 00:03:30,404 Now, what about the engine? 72 00:03:30,404 --> 00:03:32,896 The engine of the fly is absolutely fascinating. 73 00:03:32,896 --> 00:03:34,794 They have two types of flight muscle: 74 00:03:34,794 --> 00:03:37,779 so-called power muscle, which is stretch-activated, 75 00:03:37,779 --> 00:03:41,505 which means that it activates itself and does not need to be controlled 76 00:03:41,505 --> 00:03:44,844 on a contraction-by-contraction basis by the nervous system. 77 00:03:44,844 --> 00:03:49,453 It's specialized to generate the enormous power required for flight, 78 00:03:49,453 --> 00:03:51,532 and it fills the middle portion of the fly, 79 00:03:51,532 --> 00:03:53,079 so when a fly hits your windshield, 80 00:03:53,079 --> 00:03:55,485 it's basically the power muscle that you're looking at. 81 00:03:55,485 --> 00:03:57,631 But attached to the base of the wing 82 00:03:57,631 --> 00:04:00,269 is a set of little, tiny control muscles 83 00:04:00,269 --> 00:04:03,570 that are not very powerful at all, but they're very fast, 84 00:04:03,570 --> 00:04:06,776 and they're able to reconfigure the hinge of the wing 85 00:04:06,776 --> 00:04:08,538 on a stroke-by-stroke basis, 86 00:04:08,538 --> 00:04:11,680 and this is what enables the fly to change its wing 87 00:04:11,680 --> 00:04:14,651 and generate the changes in aerodynamic forces 88 00:04:14,651 --> 00:04:17,224 which change its flight trajectory. 89 00:04:17,224 --> 00:04:20,787 And of course, the role of the nervous system is to control all this. 90 00:04:20,787 --> 00:04:22,299 So let's look at the controller. 91 00:04:22,299 --> 00:04:24,946 Now flies excel in the sorts of sensors 92 00:04:24,946 --> 00:04:27,230 that they carry to this problem. 93 00:04:27,230 --> 00:04:31,357 They have antennae that sense odors and detect wind detection. 94 00:04:31,357 --> 00:04:33,032 They have a sophisticated eye which is 95 00:04:33,032 --> 00:04:35,488 the fastest visual system on the planet. 96 00:04:35,488 --> 00:04:37,524 They have another set of eyes on the top of their head. 97 00:04:37,524 --> 00:04:39,576 We have no idea what they do. 98 00:04:39,576 --> 00:04:42,530 They have sensors on their wing. 99 00:04:42,530 --> 00:04:46,290 Their wing is covered with sensors, including sensors 100 00:04:46,290 --> 00:04:48,336 that sense deformation of the wing. 101 00:04:48,336 --> 00:04:50,445 They can even taste with their wings. 102 00:04:50,445 --> 00:04:53,000 One of the most sophisticated sensors a fly has 103 00:04:53,000 --> 00:04:54,807 is a structure called the halteres. 104 00:04:54,807 --> 00:04:56,686 The halteres are actually gyroscopes. 105 00:04:56,686 --> 00:05:01,135 These devices beat back and forth about 200 hertz during flight, 106 00:05:01,135 --> 00:05:03,808 and the animal can use them to sense its body rotation 107 00:05:03,808 --> 00:05:07,776 and initiate very, very fast corrective maneuvers. 108 00:05:07,776 --> 00:05:10,105 But all of this sensory information has to be processed 109 00:05:10,105 --> 00:05:13,825 by a brain, and yes, indeed, flies have a brain, 110 00:05:13,825 --> 00:05:16,984 a brain of about 100,000 neurons. 111 00:05:16,984 --> 00:05:19,177 Now several people at this conference 112 00:05:19,177 --> 00:05:23,985 have already suggested that fruit flies could serve neuroscience 113 00:05:23,985 --> 00:05:27,232 because they're a simple model of brain function. 114 00:05:27,232 --> 00:05:29,309 And the basic punchline of my talk is, 115 00:05:29,309 --> 00:05:31,967 I'd like to turn that over on its head. 116 00:05:31,967 --> 00:05:34,595 I don't think they're a simple model of anything. 117 00:05:34,595 --> 00:05:37,072 And I think that flies are a great model. 118 00:05:37,072 --> 00:05:39,588 They're a great model for flies. 119 00:05:39,588 --> 00:05:42,069 (Laughter) 120 00:05:42,069 --> 00:05:45,072 And let's explore this notion of simplicity. 121 00:05:45,072 --> 00:05:47,503 So I think, unfortunately, a lot of neuroscientists, 122 00:05:47,503 --> 00:05:49,335 we're all somewhat narcissistic. 123 00:05:49,335 --> 00:05:52,768 When we think of brain, we of course imagine our own brain. 124 00:05:52,768 --> 00:05:54,728 But remember that this kind of brain, 125 00:05:54,728 --> 00:05:56,496 which is much, much smaller 126 00:05:56,496 --> 00:05:59,174 — instead of 100 billion neurons, it has 100,000 neurons — 127 00:05:59,174 --> 00:06:02,056 but this is the most common form of brain on the planet 128 00:06:02,056 --> 00:06:04,960 and has been for 400 million years. 129 00:06:04,960 --> 00:06:07,248 And is it fair to say that it's simple? 130 00:06:07,248 --> 00:06:09,343 Well, it's simple in the sense that it has fewer neurons, 131 00:06:09,343 --> 00:06:11,097 but is that a fair metric? 132 00:06:11,097 --> 00:06:13,373 And I would propose it's not a fair metric. 133 00:06:13,373 --> 00:06:16,473 So let's sort of think about this. I think we have to compare -- 134 00:06:16,473 --> 00:06:18,032 (Laughter) — 135 00:06:18,032 --> 00:06:23,153 we have to compare the size of the brain 136 00:06:23,153 --> 00:06:25,183 with what the brain can do. 137 00:06:25,183 --> 00:06:28,064 So I propose we have a Trump number, 138 00:06:28,064 --> 00:06:30,929 and the Trump number is the ratio of this man's 139 00:06:30,929 --> 00:06:34,608 behavioral repertoire to the number of neurons in his brain. 140 00:06:34,608 --> 00:06:37,276 We'll calculate the Trump number for the fruit fly. 141 00:06:37,276 --> 00:06:39,960 Now, how many people here think the Trump number 142 00:06:39,960 --> 00:06:42,449 is higher for the fruit fly? 143 00:06:42,449 --> 00:06:44,880 (Applause) 144 00:06:44,880 --> 00:06:48,308 It's a very smart, smart audience. 145 00:06:48,308 --> 00:06:51,635 Yes, the inequality goes in this direction, or I would posit it. 146 00:06:51,635 --> 00:06:54,017 Now I realize that it is a little bit absurd 147 00:06:54,017 --> 00:06:57,575 to compare the behavioral repertoire of a human to a fly. 148 00:06:57,575 --> 00:07:01,718 But let's take another animal just as an example. Here's a mouse. 149 00:07:01,718 --> 00:07:06,023 A mouse has about 1,000 times as many neurons as a fly. 150 00:07:06,023 --> 00:07:08,050 I used to study mice. When I studied mice, 151 00:07:08,050 --> 00:07:10,887 I used to talk really slowly. 152 00:07:10,887 --> 00:07:13,463 And then something happened when I started to work on flies. 153 00:07:13,463 --> 00:07:15,875 (Laughter) 154 00:07:15,875 --> 00:07:19,335 And I think if you compare the natural history of flies and mice, 155 00:07:19,335 --> 00:07:22,648 it's really comparable. They have to forage for food. 156 00:07:22,648 --> 00:07:25,095 They have to engage in courtship. 157 00:07:25,095 --> 00:07:28,566 They have sex. They hide from predators. 158 00:07:28,566 --> 00:07:30,546 They do a lot of the similar things. 159 00:07:30,546 --> 00:07:32,264 But I would argue that flies do more. 160 00:07:32,264 --> 00:07:35,642 So for example, I'm going to show you a sequence, 161 00:07:35,642 --> 00:07:39,847 and I have to say, some of my funding comes from the military, 162 00:07:39,847 --> 00:07:41,919 so I'm showing this classified sequence 163 00:07:41,919 --> 00:07:46,012 and you cannot discuss it outside of this room. Okay? 164 00:07:46,012 --> 00:07:47,920 So I want you to look at the payload 165 00:07:47,920 --> 00:07:50,946 at the tail of the fruit fly. 166 00:07:50,946 --> 00:07:53,047 Watch it very closely, 167 00:07:53,047 --> 00:07:57,344 and you'll see why my six-year-old son 168 00:07:57,344 --> 00:08:02,073 now wants to be a neuroscientist. 169 00:08:02,073 --> 00:08:03,252 Wait for it. 170 00:08:03,252 --> 00:08:04,821 Pshhew. 171 00:08:04,821 --> 00:08:07,905 So at least you'll admit that if fruit flies are not as clever as mice, 172 00:08:07,905 --> 00:08:12,821 they're at least as clever as pigeons. (Laughter) 173 00:08:12,821 --> 00:08:16,788 Now, I want to get across that it's not just a matter of numbers 174 00:08:16,788 --> 00:08:19,386 but also the challenge for a fly to compute 175 00:08:19,386 --> 00:08:22,235 everything its brain has to compute with such tiny neurons. 176 00:08:22,235 --> 00:08:25,223 So this is a beautiful image of a visual interneuron from a mouse 177 00:08:25,223 --> 00:08:27,991 that came from Jeff Lichtman's lab, 178 00:08:27,991 --> 00:08:31,238 and you can see the wonderful images of brains 179 00:08:31,238 --> 00:08:34,431 that he showed in his talk. 180 00:08:34,431 --> 00:08:36,799 But up in the corner, in the right corner, you'll see, 181 00:08:36,799 --> 00:08:40,911 at the same scale, a visual interneuron from a fly. 182 00:08:40,911 --> 00:08:42,752 And I'll expand this up. 183 00:08:42,752 --> 00:08:44,922 And it's a beautifully complex neuron. 184 00:08:44,922 --> 00:08:48,407 It's just very, very tiny, and there's lots of biophysical challenges 185 00:08:48,407 --> 00:08:52,030 with trying to compute information with tiny, tiny neurons. 186 00:08:52,030 --> 00:08:55,567 How small can neurons get? Well, look at this interesting insect. 187 00:08:55,567 --> 00:08:57,779 It looks sort of like a fly. It has wings, it has eyes, 188 00:08:57,779 --> 00:09:00,578 it has antennae, its legs, complicated life history, 189 00:09:00,578 --> 00:09:03,674 it's a parasite, it has to fly around and find caterpillars 190 00:09:03,674 --> 00:09:05,056 to parasatize, 191 00:09:05,056 --> 00:09:09,171 but not only is its brain the size of a salt grain, 192 00:09:09,171 --> 00:09:11,140 which is comparable for a fruit fly, 193 00:09:11,140 --> 00:09:14,066 it is the size of a salt grain. 194 00:09:14,066 --> 00:09:17,701 So here's some other organisms at the similar scale. 195 00:09:17,701 --> 00:09:21,831 This animal is the size of a paramecium and an amoeba, 196 00:09:21,831 --> 00:09:25,711 and it has a brain of 7,000 neurons that's so small -- 197 00:09:25,711 --> 00:09:28,167 you know these things called cell bodies you've been hearing about, 198 00:09:28,167 --> 00:09:29,818 where the nucleus of the neuron is? 199 00:09:29,818 --> 00:09:33,278 This animal gets rid of them because they take up too much space. 200 00:09:33,278 --> 00:09:35,751 So this is a session on frontiers in neuroscience. 201 00:09:35,751 --> 00:09:41,111 I would posit that one frontier in neuroscience is to figure out how the brain of that thing works. 202 00:09:41,111 --> 00:09:46,744 But let's think about this. How can you make a small number of neurons do a lot? 203 00:09:46,744 --> 00:09:49,266 And I think, from an engineering perspective, 204 00:09:49,266 --> 00:09:50,995 you think of multiplexing. 205 00:09:50,995 --> 00:09:53,698 You can take a hardware and have that hardware 206 00:09:53,698 --> 00:09:55,311 do different things at different times, 207 00:09:55,311 --> 00:09:58,306 or have different parts of the hardware doing different things. 208 00:09:58,306 --> 00:10:01,577 And these are the two concepts I'd like to explore. 209 00:10:01,577 --> 00:10:03,235 And they're not concepts that I've come up with, 210 00:10:03,235 --> 00:10:07,780 but concepts that have been proposed by others in the past. 211 00:10:07,780 --> 00:10:10,855 And one idea comes from lessons from chewing crabs. 212 00:10:10,855 --> 00:10:12,722 And I don't mean chewing the crabs. 213 00:10:12,722 --> 00:10:16,321 I grew up in Baltimore, and I chew crabs very, very well. 214 00:10:16,321 --> 00:10:19,178 But I'm talking about the crabs actually doing the chewing. 215 00:10:19,178 --> 00:10:21,208 Crab chewing is actually really fascinating. 216 00:10:21,208 --> 00:10:24,467 Crabs have this complicated structure under their carapace 217 00:10:24,467 --> 00:10:25,777 called the gastric mill 218 00:10:25,777 --> 00:10:28,207 that grinds their food in a variety of different ways. 219 00:10:28,207 --> 00:10:33,466 And here's an endoscopic movie of this structure. 220 00:10:33,466 --> 00:10:36,026 The amazing thing about this is that it's controlled 221 00:10:36,026 --> 00:10:39,458 by a really tiny set of neurons, about two dozen neurons 222 00:10:39,458 --> 00:10:44,421 that can produce a vast variety of different motor patterns, 223 00:10:44,421 --> 00:10:48,768 and the reason it can do this is that this little tiny ganglion 224 00:10:48,768 --> 00:10:52,952 in the crab is actually inundated by many, many neuromodulators. 225 00:10:52,952 --> 00:10:55,093 You heard about neuromodulators earlier. 226 00:10:55,093 --> 00:10:57,318 There are more neuromodulators 227 00:10:57,318 --> 00:11:02,803 that alter, that innervate this structure than actually neurons in the structure, 228 00:11:02,803 --> 00:11:07,045 and they're able to generate a complicated set of patterns. 229 00:11:07,045 --> 00:11:10,486 And this is the work by Eve Marder and her many colleagues 230 00:11:10,486 --> 00:11:12,781 who've been studying this fascinating system 231 00:11:12,781 --> 00:11:14,933 that show how a smaller cluster of neurons 232 00:11:14,933 --> 00:11:16,758 can do many, many, many things 233 00:11:16,758 --> 00:11:21,614 because of neuromodulation that can take place on a moment-by-moment basis. 234 00:11:21,614 --> 00:11:24,053 So this is basically multiplexing in time. 235 00:11:24,053 --> 00:11:26,838 Imagine a network of neurons with one neuromodulator. 236 00:11:26,838 --> 00:11:30,316 You select one set of cells to perform one sort of behavior, 237 00:11:30,316 --> 00:11:32,934 another neuromodulator, another set of cells, 238 00:11:32,934 --> 00:11:34,647 a different pattern, and you can imagine 239 00:11:34,647 --> 00:11:38,525 you could extrapolate to a very, very complicated system. 240 00:11:38,525 --> 00:11:40,619 Is there any evidence that flies do this? 241 00:11:40,619 --> 00:11:43,994 Well, for many years in my laboratory and other laboratories around the world, 242 00:11:43,994 --> 00:11:46,642 we've been studying fly behaviors in little flight simulators. 243 00:11:46,642 --> 00:11:48,348 You can tether a fly to a little stick. 244 00:11:48,348 --> 00:11:50,849 You can measure the aerodynamic forces it's creating. 245 00:11:50,849 --> 00:11:53,395 You can let the fly play a little video game 246 00:11:53,395 --> 00:11:57,273 by letting it fly around in a visual display. 247 00:11:57,273 --> 00:11:59,610 So let me show you a little tiny sequence of this. 248 00:11:59,610 --> 00:12:00,837 Here's a fly 249 00:12:00,837 --> 00:12:04,274 and a large infrared view of the fly in the flight simulator, 250 00:12:04,274 --> 00:12:06,229 and this is a game the flies love to play. 251 00:12:06,229 --> 00:12:08,666 You allow them to steer towards the little stripe, 252 00:12:08,666 --> 00:12:11,491 and they'll just steer towards that stripe forever. 253 00:12:11,491 --> 00:12:15,049 It's part of their visual guidance system. 254 00:12:15,049 --> 00:12:17,394 But very, very recently, it's been possible 255 00:12:17,394 --> 00:12:22,334 to modify these sorts of behavioral arenas for physiologies. 256 00:12:22,334 --> 00:12:24,822 So this is the preparation that one of my former post-docs, 257 00:12:24,822 --> 00:12:27,265 Gaby Maimon, who's now at Rockefeller, developed, 258 00:12:27,265 --> 00:12:28,951 and it's basically a flight simulator 259 00:12:28,951 --> 00:12:32,026 but under conditions where you actually can stick an electrode 260 00:12:32,026 --> 00:12:34,290 in the brain of the fly and record 261 00:12:34,290 --> 00:12:37,946 from a genetically identified neuron in the fly's brain. 262 00:12:37,946 --> 00:12:40,244 And this is what one of these experiments looks like. 263 00:12:40,244 --> 00:12:43,215 It was a sequence taken from another post-doc in the lab, 264 00:12:43,215 --> 00:12:44,414 Bettina Schnell. 265 00:12:44,414 --> 00:12:47,806 The green trace at the bottom is the membrane potential 266 00:12:47,806 --> 00:12:49,836 of a neuron in the fly's brain, 267 00:12:49,836 --> 00:12:52,778 and you'll see the fly start to fly, and the fly is actually 268 00:12:52,778 --> 00:12:56,057 controlling the rotation of that visual pattern itself 269 00:12:56,057 --> 00:12:57,536 by its own wing motion, 270 00:12:57,536 --> 00:12:59,646 and you can see this visual interneuron 271 00:12:59,646 --> 00:13:03,554 respond to the pattern of wing motion as the fly flies. 272 00:13:03,554 --> 00:13:05,930 So for the first time we've actually been able to record 273 00:13:05,930 --> 00:13:08,838 from neurons in the fly's brain while the fly 274 00:13:08,838 --> 00:13:13,306 is performing sophisticated behaviors such as flight. 275 00:13:13,306 --> 00:13:15,161 And one of the lessons we've been learning 276 00:13:15,161 --> 00:13:17,581 is that the physiology of cells that we've been studying 277 00:13:17,581 --> 00:13:20,002 for many years in quiescent flies 278 00:13:20,002 --> 00:13:22,650 is not the same as the physiology of those cells 279 00:13:22,650 --> 00:13:25,386 when the flies actually engage in active behaviors 280 00:13:25,386 --> 00:13:27,925 like flying and walking and so forth. 281 00:13:27,925 --> 00:13:30,850 And why is the physiology different? 282 00:13:30,850 --> 00:13:32,907 Well it turns out it's these neuromodulators, 283 00:13:32,907 --> 00:13:36,858 just like the neuromodulators in that little tiny ganglion in the crabs. 284 00:13:36,858 --> 00:13:39,408 So here's a picture of the octopamine system. 285 00:13:39,408 --> 00:13:41,162 Octopamine is a neuromodulator 286 00:13:41,162 --> 00:13:45,498 that seems to play an important role in flight and other behaviors. 287 00:13:45,498 --> 00:13:47,970 But this is just one of many neuromodulators 288 00:13:47,970 --> 00:13:49,041 that's in the fly's brain. 289 00:13:49,041 --> 00:13:51,707 So I really think that, as we learn more, 290 00:13:51,707 --> 00:13:54,234 it's going to turn out that the whole fly brain 291 00:13:54,234 --> 00:13:57,323 is just like a large version of this stomatogastric ganglion, 292 00:13:57,323 --> 00:14:01,683 and that's one of the reasons why it can do so much with so few neurons. 293 00:14:01,683 --> 00:14:04,470 Now, another idea, another way of multiplexing 294 00:14:04,470 --> 00:14:06,126 is multiplexing in space, 295 00:14:06,126 --> 00:14:07,820 having different parts of a neuron 296 00:14:07,820 --> 00:14:09,942 do different things at the same time. 297 00:14:09,942 --> 00:14:11,775 So here's two sort of canonical neurons 298 00:14:11,775 --> 00:14:14,060 from a vertebrate and an invertebrate, 299 00:14:14,060 --> 00:14:17,310 a human pyramidal neuron from Ramon y Cajal, 300 00:14:17,310 --> 00:14:21,313 and another cell to the right, a non-spiking interneuron, 301 00:14:21,313 --> 00:14:25,460 and this is the work of Alan Watson and Malcolm Burrows many years ago, 302 00:14:25,460 --> 00:14:28,535 and Malcolm Burrows came up with a pretty interesting idea 303 00:14:28,535 --> 00:14:31,417 based on the fact that this neuron from a locust 304 00:14:31,417 --> 00:14:33,376 does not fire action potentials. 305 00:14:33,376 --> 00:14:35,124 It's a non-spiking cell. 306 00:14:35,124 --> 00:14:37,904 So a typical cell, like the neurons in our brain, 307 00:14:37,904 --> 00:14:40,656 has a region called the dendrites that receives input, 308 00:14:40,656 --> 00:14:43,245 and that input sums together 309 00:14:43,245 --> 00:14:45,541 and will produce action potentials 310 00:14:45,541 --> 00:14:47,872 that run down the axon and then activate 311 00:14:47,872 --> 00:14:50,168 all the output regions of the neuron. 312 00:14:50,168 --> 00:14:53,044 But non-spiking neurons are actually quite complicated 313 00:14:53,044 --> 00:14:56,156 because they can have input synapses and output synapses 314 00:14:56,156 --> 00:14:59,819 all interdigitated, and there's no single action potential 315 00:14:59,819 --> 00:15:02,945 that drives all the outputs at the same time. 316 00:15:02,945 --> 00:15:06,852 So there's a possibility that you have computational compartments 317 00:15:06,852 --> 00:15:10,830 that allow the different parts of the neuron 318 00:15:10,830 --> 00:15:13,390 to do different things at the same time. 319 00:15:13,390 --> 00:15:18,061 So these basic concepts of multitasking in time 320 00:15:18,061 --> 00:15:20,422 and multitasking in space, 321 00:15:20,422 --> 00:15:23,254 I think these are things that are true in our brains as well, 322 00:15:23,254 --> 00:15:25,831 but I think the insects are the true masters of this. 323 00:15:25,831 --> 00:15:28,947 So I hope you think of insects a little bit differently next time, 324 00:15:28,947 --> 00:15:31,882 and as I say up here, please think before you swat. 325 00:15:31,882 --> 00:15:34,835 (Applause)