Return to Video

The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain

  • 0:01 - 0:04
    Fifteen years ago, it was widely assumed
  • 0:04 - 0:06
    that the vast majority of brain development
  • 0:06 - 0:09
    takes place in the first few years of life.
  • 0:09 - 0:11
    Back then, 15 years ago, we didn't have the ability
  • 0:11 - 0:14
    to look inside the living human brain
  • 0:14 - 0:17
    and track development across the lifespan.
  • 0:17 - 0:20
    In the past decade or so, mainly due to advances
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    in brain imaging technology
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    such as magnetic resonance imaging, or MRI,
  • 0:25 - 0:28
    neuroscientists have started to look inside the living
  • 0:28 - 0:30
    human brain of all ages, and to track changes
  • 0:30 - 0:33
    in brain structure and brain function,
  • 0:33 - 0:37
    so we use structural MRI if you'd like to take a snapshot,
  • 0:37 - 0:40
    a photograph, at really high resolution of the inside
  • 0:40 - 0:43
    of the living human brain, and we can ask questions like,
  • 0:43 - 0:46
    how much gray matter does the brain contain,
  • 0:46 - 0:48
    and how does that change with age?
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    And we also use functional MRI, called fMRI,
  • 0:51 - 0:55
    to take a video, a movie, of brain activity
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    when participants are taking part in some kind of task
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    like thinking or feeling or perceiving something.
  • 1:00 - 1:03
    So many labs around the world are involved in this kind
  • 1:03 - 1:05
    of research, and we now have a really rich
  • 1:05 - 1:09
    and detailed picture of how the living human brain develops,
  • 1:09 - 1:12
    and this picture has radically changed the way
  • 1:12 - 1:15
    we think about human brain development
  • 1:15 - 1:17
    by revealing that it's not all over in early childhood,
  • 1:17 - 1:20
    and instead, the brain continues to develop
  • 1:20 - 1:24
    right throughout adolescence and into the '20s and '30s.
  • 1:24 - 1:28
    So adolescence is defined as the period of life that starts
  • 1:28 - 1:32
    with the biological, hormonal, physical changes of puberty
  • 1:32 - 1:36
    and ends at the age at which an individual attains
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    a stable, independent role in society.
  • 1:39 - 1:41
    (Laughter)
  • 1:41 - 1:44
    It can go on a long time. (Laughter)
  • 1:44 - 1:47
    One of the brain regions that changes most dramatically
  • 1:47 - 1:50
    during adolescence is called prefrontal cortex.
  • 1:50 - 1:53
    So this is a model of the human brain,
  • 1:53 - 1:56
    and this is prefrontal cortex, right at the front.
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    Prefrontal cortex is an interesting brain area.
  • 1:58 - 2:02
    It's proportionally much bigger in humans than
  • 2:02 - 2:05
    in any other species, and it's involved in a whole range of
  • 2:05 - 2:08
    high level cognitive functions, things like decision-making,
  • 2:08 - 2:11
    planning, planning what you're going to do tomorrow
  • 2:11 - 2:13
    or next week or next year, inhibiting
  • 2:13 - 2:16
    inappropriate behavior, so stopping yourself saying
  • 2:16 - 2:19
    something really rude or doing something really stupid.
  • 2:19 - 2:21
    It's also involved in social interaction,
  • 2:21 - 2:24
    understanding other people, and self-awareness.
  • 2:24 - 2:27
    So MRI studies looking at the development of this region
  • 2:27 - 2:29
    have shown that it really undergoes dramatic development
  • 2:29 - 2:32
    during the period of adolescence.
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    So if you look at gray matter volume, for example,
  • 2:35 - 2:40
    gray matter volume across age from age four to 22 years
  • 2:40 - 2:42
    increases during childhood, which is what you can see
  • 2:42 - 2:46
    on this graph. It peaks in early adolescence.
  • 2:46 - 2:49
    The arrows indicate peak gray matter volume
  • 2:49 - 2:52
    in prefrontal cortex. You can see that that peak happens
  • 2:52 - 2:56
    a couple of years later in boys relative to girls,
  • 2:56 - 2:58
    and that's probably because boys go through puberty
  • 2:58 - 3:00
    a couple of years later than girls on average,
  • 3:00 - 3:03
    and then during adolescence, there's a significant decline
  • 3:03 - 3:06
    in gray matter volume in prefrontal cortex.
  • 3:06 - 3:08
    Now that might sound bad, but actually this is
  • 3:08 - 3:11
    a really important developmental process, because
  • 3:11 - 3:15
    gray matter contains cell bodies and connections
  • 3:15 - 3:19
    between cells, the synapses, and this decline
  • 3:19 - 3:21
    in gray matter volume during prefrontal cortex
  • 3:21 - 3:24
    is thought to correspond to synaptic pruning,
  • 3:24 - 3:27
    the elimination of unwanted synapses.
  • 3:27 - 3:30
    This is a really important process. It's partly dependent
  • 3:30 - 3:33
    on the environment that the animal or the human is in,
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    and the synapses that are being used are strengthened,
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    and synapses that aren't being used
  • 3:38 - 3:41
    in that particular environment are pruned away.
  • 3:41 - 3:43
    You can think of it a bit like pruning a rosebush.
  • 3:43 - 3:46
    You prune away the weaker branches so that
  • 3:46 - 3:49
    the remaining, important branches, can grow stronger,
  • 3:49 - 3:53
    and this process, which effectively fine-tunes brain tissue
  • 3:53 - 3:56
    according to the species-specific environment,
  • 3:56 - 3:58
    is happening in prefrontal cortex and in other brain regions
  • 3:58 - 4:02
    during the period of human adolescence.
  • 4:02 - 4:06
    So a second line of inquiry that we use to track changes
  • 4:06 - 4:09
    in the adolescent brain is using functional MRI
  • 4:09 - 4:12
    to look at changes in brain activity across age.
  • 4:12 - 4:14
    So I'll just give you an example from my lab.
  • 4:14 - 4:17
    So in my lab, we're interested in the social brain, that is
  • 4:17 - 4:21
    the network of brain regions that we use to understand
  • 4:21 - 4:23
    other people and to interact with other people.
  • 4:23 - 4:27
    So I like to show a photograph of a soccer game
  • 4:27 - 4:32
    to illustrate two aspects of how your social brains work.
  • 4:32 - 4:33
    So this is a soccer game. (Laughter)
  • 4:33 - 4:36
    Michael Owen has just missed a goal, and he's lying
  • 4:36 - 4:38
    on the ground, and the first aspect of the social brain
  • 4:38 - 4:41
    that this picture really nicely illustrates is how automatic
  • 4:41 - 4:44
    and instinctive social emotional responses are,
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    so within a split second of Michael Owen missing this goal,
  • 4:47 - 4:49
    everyone is doing the same thing with their arms
  • 4:49 - 4:51
    and the same thing with their face, even Michael Owen
  • 4:51 - 4:53
    as he slides along the grass, is doing the same thing
  • 4:53 - 4:55
    with his arms, and presumably has a similar
  • 4:55 - 4:57
    facial expression, and the only people who don't
  • 4:57 - 5:01
    are the guys in yellow at the back — (Laughs) —
  • 5:01 - 5:03
    and I think they're on the wrong end of the stadium,
  • 5:03 - 5:05
    and they're doing another social emotional response
  • 5:05 - 5:08
    that we all instantly recognize, and that's the second aspect
  • 5:08 - 5:12
    of the social brain that this picture really nicely illustrates,
  • 5:12 - 5:15
    how good we are at reading other people's behavior,
  • 5:15 - 5:18
    their actions, their gestures, their facial expressions,
  • 5:18 - 5:22
    in terms of their underlying emotions and mental states.
  • 5:22 - 5:23
    So you don't have to ask any of these guys.
  • 5:23 - 5:26
    You have a pretty good idea of what they're feeling
  • 5:26 - 5:28
    and thinking at this precise moment in time.
  • 5:28 - 5:30
    So that's what we're interested in looking at in my lab.
  • 5:30 - 5:34
    So in my lab, we bring adolescents and adults into the lab
  • 5:34 - 5:36
    to have a brain scan, we give them some kind of task
  • 5:36 - 5:40
    that involves thinking about other people, their minds,
  • 5:40 - 5:42
    their mental states, their emotions, and one of the findings
  • 5:42 - 5:45
    that we've found several times now, as have other labs
  • 5:45 - 5:48
    around the world, is part of the prefrontal cortex called
  • 5:48 - 5:52
    medial prefrontal cortex, which is shown in blue on the slide,
  • 5:52 - 5:54
    and it's right in the middle of prefrontal cortex
  • 5:54 - 5:57
    in the midline of your head.
  • 5:57 - 6:00
    This region is more active in adolescents when they make
  • 6:00 - 6:02
    these social decisions and think about other people
  • 6:02 - 6:05
    than it is in adults, and this is actually a meta-analysis
  • 6:05 - 6:08
    of nine different studies in this area from labs around
  • 6:08 - 6:11
    the world, and they all show the same thing, that activity
  • 6:11 - 6:14
    in this medial prefrontal cortex area decreases
  • 6:14 - 6:17
    during the period of adolescence.
  • 6:17 - 6:19
    And we think that might be because adolescents and adults
  • 6:19 - 6:22
    use a different mental approach, a different
  • 6:22 - 6:25
    cognitive strategy, to make social decisions,
  • 6:25 - 6:29
    and one way of looking at that is to do behavioral studies
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    whereby we bring people into the lab and we give them
  • 6:31 - 6:33
    some kind of behavioral task, and I'll just give you
  • 6:33 - 6:36
    another example of the kind of task that we use in my lab.
  • 6:36 - 6:39
    So imagine that you're the participant in one of our
  • 6:39 - 6:41
    experiments. You come into the lab,
  • 6:41 - 6:44
    you see this computerized task.
  • 6:44 - 6:46
    In this task, you see a set of shelves.
  • 6:46 - 6:49
    Now, there are objects on these shelves, on some of them,
  • 6:49 - 6:52
    and you'll notice there's a guy standing behind the set
  • 6:52 - 6:56
    of shelves, and there are some objects that he can't see.
  • 6:56 - 6:58
    They're occluded, from his point of view, with a kind of
  • 6:58 - 7:01
    gray piece of wood.
  • 7:01 - 7:04
    This is the same set of shelves from his point of view.
  • 7:04 - 7:08
    Notice that there are only some objects that he can see,
  • 7:08 - 7:10
    whereas there are many more objects that you can see.
  • 7:10 - 7:12
    Now your task is to move objects around.
  • 7:12 - 7:15
    The director, standing behind the set of shelves,
  • 7:15 - 7:17
    is going to direct you to move objects around,
  • 7:17 - 7:19
    but remember, he's not going to ask you to move objects
  • 7:19 - 7:23
    that he can't see. This introduces a really interesting
  • 7:23 - 7:25
    condition whereby there's a kind of conflict
  • 7:25 - 7:28
    between your perspective and the director's perspective.
  • 7:28 - 7:32
    So imagine he tells you to move the top truck left.
  • 7:32 - 7:34
    There are three trucks there. You're going to instinctively
  • 7:34 - 7:36
    go for the white truck, because that's the top truck
  • 7:36 - 7:39
    from your perspective, but then you have to remember,
  • 7:39 - 7:41
    "Oh, he can't see that truck, so he must mean
  • 7:41 - 7:43
    me to move the blue truck," which is the top truck
  • 7:43 - 7:46
    from his perspective. Now believe it or not,
  • 7:46 - 7:49
    normal, healthy, intelligent adults like you make errors
  • 7:49 - 7:52
    about 50 percent of the time on that kind of trial.
  • 7:52 - 7:55
    They move the white truck instead of the blue truck.
  • 7:55 - 7:58
    So we give this kind of task to adolescents and adults,
  • 7:58 - 8:00
    and we also have a control condition
  • 8:00 - 8:04
    where there's no director and instead we give people a rule.
  • 8:04 - 8:06
    We tell them, okay, we're going to do exactly the same thing
  • 8:06 - 8:09
    but this time there's no director. Instead you've got to
  • 8:09 - 8:12
    ignore objects with the dark gray background.
  • 8:12 - 8:15
    You'll see that this is exactly the same condition, only
  • 8:15 - 8:17
    in the no-director condition they just have to remember
  • 8:17 - 8:20
    to apply this somewhat arbitrary rule, whereas
  • 8:20 - 8:22
    in the director condition, they have to remember
  • 8:22 - 8:26
    to take into account the director's perspective
  • 8:26 - 8:30
    in order to guide their ongoing behavior.
  • 8:30 - 8:33
    Okay, so if I just show you the percentage errors
  • 8:33 - 8:35
    in a large developmental study we did,
  • 8:35 - 8:39
    this is in a study ranging from age seven to adulthood,
  • 8:39 - 8:40
    and what you're going to see is the percentage errors
  • 8:40 - 8:42
    in the adult group in both conditions,
  • 8:42 - 8:45
    so the gray is the director condition, and you see
  • 8:45 - 8:48
    that our intelligent adults are making errors about 50 percent
  • 8:48 - 8:50
    of the time, whereas they make far fewer errors
  • 8:50 - 8:53
    when there's no director present, when they just have
  • 8:53 - 8:56
    to remember that rule of ignoring the gray background.
  • 8:56 - 8:58
    Developmentally, these two conditions develop
  • 8:58 - 9:01
    in exactly the same way. Between late childhood
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    and mid-adolescence, there's an improvement,
  • 9:03 - 9:07
    in other words a reduction of errors, in both of these trials,
  • 9:07 - 9:08
    in both of these conditions.
  • 9:08 - 9:10
    But it's when you compare the last two groups,
  • 9:10 - 9:12
    the mid-adolescent group and the adult group
  • 9:12 - 9:15
    where things get really interesting, because there, there is
  • 9:15 - 9:18
    no continued improvement in the no-director condition.
  • 9:18 - 9:21
    In other words, everything you need to do in order to
  • 9:21 - 9:24
    remember the rule and apply it seems to be fully developed
  • 9:24 - 9:26
    by mid-adolescence, whereas in contrast,
  • 9:26 - 9:29
    if you look at the last two gray bars, there's still
  • 9:29 - 9:32
    a significant improvement in the director condition
  • 9:32 - 9:34
    between mid-adolescence and adulthood, and what
  • 9:34 - 9:38
    this means is that the ability to take into account someone
  • 9:38 - 9:41
    else's perspective in order to guide ongoing behavior,
  • 9:41 - 9:43
    which is something, by the way, that we do in everyday life all
  • 9:43 - 9:48
    the time, is still developing in mid-to-late adolescence.
  • 9:48 - 9:51
    So if you have a teenage son or a daughter and you
  • 9:51 - 9:53
    sometimes think they have problems taking other people's
  • 9:53 - 9:57
    perspectives, you're right. They do. And this is why.
  • 9:57 - 10:00
    So we sometimes laugh about teenagers.
  • 10:00 - 10:04
    They're parodied, sometimes even demonized in the media
  • 10:04 - 10:08
    for their kind of typical teenage behavior. They take risks,
  • 10:08 - 10:10
    they're sometimes moody, they're very self-conscious.
  • 10:10 - 10:13
    I have a really nice anecdote from a friend of mine
  • 10:13 - 10:15
    who said that the thing he noticed most
  • 10:15 - 10:18
    about his teenage daughters before and after puberty
  • 10:18 - 10:21
    was their level of embarrassment in front of him.
  • 10:21 - 10:23
    So, he said, "Before puberty, if my two daughters
  • 10:23 - 10:25
    were messing around in a shop, I'd say, 'Hey,
  • 10:25 - 10:27
    stop messing around and I'll sing your favorite song,'
  • 10:27 - 10:29
    and instantly they'd stop messing around and he'd sing
  • 10:29 - 10:32
    their favorite song. After puberty, that became the threat.
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    (Laughter)
  • 10:34 - 10:38
    The very notion of their dad singing in public
  • 10:38 - 10:40
    was enough to make them behave.
  • 10:40 - 10:42
    So people often ask,
  • 10:42 - 10:44
    "Well, is adolescence a kind of recent phenomenon?
  • 10:44 - 10:46
    Is it something we've invented recently in the West?"
  • 10:46 - 10:49
    And actually, the answer is probably not. There are lots
  • 10:49 - 10:52
    of descriptions of adolescence in history that sound
  • 10:52 - 10:55
    very similar to the descriptions we use today.
  • 10:55 - 10:59
    So there's a famous quote by Shakespeare from "The Winter's Tale"
  • 10:59 - 11:02
    where he describes adolescence as follows:
  • 11:02 - 11:04
    "I would there were no age between ten and
  • 11:04 - 11:08
    three-and-twenty, or that youth would sleep out the rest;
  • 11:08 - 11:10
    for there is nothing in the between but getting wenches
  • 11:10 - 11:16
    with child, wronging the ancientry, stealing, fighting." (Laughter)
  • 11:16 - 11:21
    He then goes on to say, "Having said that, would any
  • 11:21 - 11:24
    but these boiled brains of nineteen and two-and-twenty
  • 11:24 - 11:26
    hunt in this weather?" (Laughter)
  • 11:26 - 11:29
    So almost 400 years ago, Shakespeare was portraying
  • 11:29 - 11:32
    adolescents in a very similar light to the light that we
  • 11:32 - 11:35
    portray them in today, but today we try to understand
  • 11:35 - 11:38
    their behavior in terms of the underlying changes
  • 11:38 - 11:40
    that are going on in their brain.
  • 11:40 - 11:44
    So for example, take risk-taking. We know that adolescents
  • 11:44 - 11:46
    have a tendency to take risks. They do.
  • 11:46 - 11:49
    They take more risks than children or adults,
  • 11:49 - 11:51
    and they are particularly prone to taking risks
  • 11:51 - 11:54
    when they're with their friends. There's an important drive
  • 11:54 - 11:57
    to become independent from one's parents
  • 11:57 - 12:00
    and to impress one's friends in adolescence.
  • 12:00 - 12:02
    But now we try to understand that in terms of
  • 12:02 - 12:05
    the development of a part of their brain called the limbic system,
  • 12:05 - 12:08
    so I'm going to show you the limbic system in red
  • 12:08 - 12:10
    in the slide behind me, and also on this brain.
  • 12:10 - 12:13
    So the limbic system is right deep inside the brain,
  • 12:13 - 12:16
    and it's involved in things like emotion processing
  • 12:16 - 12:20
    and reward processing. It gives you the rewarding feeling
  • 12:20 - 12:23
    out of doing fun things, including taking risks.
  • 12:23 - 12:25
    It gives you the kick out of taking risks.
  • 12:25 - 12:28
    And this region, the regions within the limbic system,
  • 12:28 - 12:32
    have been found to be hypersensitive to the rewarding
  • 12:32 - 12:35
    feeling of risk-taking in adolescents compared with adults,
  • 12:35 - 12:39
    and at the very same time, the prefrontal cortex,
  • 12:39 - 12:41
    which you can see in blue in the slide here,
  • 12:41 - 12:44
    which stops us taking excessive risks,
  • 12:44 - 12:48
    is still very much in development in adolescents.
  • 12:48 - 12:51
    So brain research has shown that the adolescent brain
  • 12:51 - 12:55
    undergoes really quite profound development,
  • 12:55 - 12:59
    and this has implications for education, for rehabilitation,
  • 12:59 - 13:03
    and intervention. The environment, including teaching,
  • 13:03 - 13:06
    can and does shape the developing adolescent brain,
  • 13:06 - 13:09
    and yet it's only relatively recently that we have been
  • 13:09 - 13:11
    routinely educating teenagers in the West.
  • 13:11 - 13:15
    All four of my grandparents, for example, left school
  • 13:15 - 13:19
    in their early adolescence. They had no choice.
  • 13:19 - 13:22
    And that's still the case for many, many teenagers
  • 13:22 - 13:25
    around the world today. Forty percent of teenagers
  • 13:25 - 13:29
    don't have access to secondary school education.
  • 13:29 - 13:32
    And yet, this is a period of life where the brain is
  • 13:32 - 13:35
    particularly adaptable and malleable.
  • 13:35 - 13:38
    It's a fantastic opportunity for learning and creativity.
  • 13:38 - 13:41
    So what's sometimes seen as the problem
  • 13:41 - 13:43
    with adolescents — heightened risk-taking, poor impulse
  • 13:43 - 13:47
    control, self-consciousness — shouldn't be stigmatized.
  • 13:47 - 13:50
    It actually reflects changes in the brain that provide
  • 13:50 - 13:53
    an excellent opportunity for education
  • 13:53 - 13:57
    and social development. Thank you. (Applause)
  • 13:57 - 14:05
    (Applause)
Title:
The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain
Speaker:
Sarah-Jayne Blakemore
Description:

Why do teenagers seem so much more impulsive, so much less self-aware than grown-ups? Cognitive neuroscientist Sarah-Jayne Blakemore compares the prefrontal cortex in adolescents to that of adults, to show us how typically “teenage” behavior is caused by the growing and developing brain.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
14:26

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions