Return to Video

What we're learning from 5,000 brains

  • 0:00 - 0:03
    Other people. Everyone is interested in other people.
  • 0:03 - 0:05
    Everyone has relationships with other people,
  • 0:05 - 0:07
    and they're interested in these relationships
  • 0:07 - 0:09
    for a variety of reasons.
  • 0:09 - 0:11
    Good relationships, bad relationships,
  • 0:11 - 0:14
    annoying relationships, agnostic relationships,
  • 0:14 - 0:17
    and what I'm going to do is focus on the central piece
  • 0:17 - 0:21
    of an interaction that goes on in a relationship.
  • 0:21 - 0:23
    So I'm going to take as inspiration the fact that we're all
  • 0:23 - 0:25
    interested in interacting with other people,
  • 0:25 - 0:29
    I'm going to completely strip it of all its complicating features,
  • 0:29 - 0:33
    and I'm going to turn that object, that simplified object,
  • 0:33 - 0:37
    into a scientific probe, and provide the early stages,
  • 0:37 - 0:40
    embryonic stages of new insights into what happens
  • 0:40 - 0:43
    in two brains while they simultaneously interact.
  • 0:43 - 0:46
    But before I do that, let me tell you a couple of things
  • 0:46 - 0:47
    that made this possible.
  • 0:47 - 0:50
    The first is we can now eavesdrop safely
  • 0:50 - 0:53
    on healthy brain activity.
  • 0:53 - 0:56
    Without needles and radioactivity,
  • 0:56 - 0:58
    without any kind of clinical reason, we can go down the street
  • 0:58 - 1:02
    and record from your friends' and neighbors' brains
  • 1:02 - 1:04
    while they do a variety of cognitive tasks, and we use
  • 1:04 - 1:08
    a method called functional magnetic resonance imaging.
  • 1:08 - 1:10
    You've probably all read about it or heard about in some
  • 1:10 - 1:14
    incarnation. Let me give you a two-sentence version of it.
  • 1:14 - 1:18
    So we've all heard of MRIs. MRIs use magnetic fields
  • 1:18 - 1:20
    and radio waves and they take snapshots of your brain
  • 1:20 - 1:22
    or your knee or your stomach,
  • 1:22 - 1:24
    grayscale images that are frozen in time.
  • 1:24 - 1:27
    In the 1990s, it was discovered you could use
  • 1:27 - 1:29
    the same machines in a different mode,
  • 1:29 - 1:32
    and in that mode, you could make microscopic blood flow
  • 1:32 - 1:35
    movies from hundreds of thousands of sites independently in the brain.
  • 1:35 - 1:38
    Okay, so what? In fact, the so what is, in the brain,
  • 1:38 - 1:42
    changes in neural activity, the things that make your brain work,
  • 1:42 - 1:44
    the things that make your software work in your brain,
  • 1:44 - 1:47
    are tightly correlated with changes in blood flow.
  • 1:47 - 1:49
    You make a blood flow movie, you have an independent
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    proxy of brain activity.
  • 1:51 - 1:54
    This has literally revolutionized cognitive science.
  • 1:54 - 1:56
    Take any cognitive domain you want, memory,
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    motor planning, thinking about your mother-in-law,
  • 1:58 - 2:02
    getting angry at people, emotional response, it goes on and on,
  • 2:02 - 2:05
    put people into functional MRI devices, and
  • 2:05 - 2:08
    image how these kinds of variables map onto brain activity.
  • 2:08 - 2:11
    It's in its early stages, and it's crude by some measures,
  • 2:11 - 2:14
    but in fact, 20 years ago, we were at nothing.
  • 2:14 - 2:16
    You couldn't do people like this. You couldn't do healthy people.
  • 2:16 - 2:19
    That's caused a literal revolution, and it's opened us up
  • 2:19 - 2:21
    to a new experimental preparation. Neurobiologists,
  • 2:21 - 2:25
    as you well know, have lots of experimental preps,
  • 2:25 - 2:28
    worms and rodents and fruit flies and things like this.
  • 2:28 - 2:32
    And now, we have a new experimental prep: human beings.
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    We can now use human beings to study and model
  • 2:35 - 2:38
    the software in human beings, and we have a few
  • 2:38 - 2:41
    burgeoning biological measures.
  • 2:41 - 2:45
    Okay, let me give you one example of the kinds of experiments that people do,
  • 2:45 - 2:48
    and it's in the area of what you'd call valuation.
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    Valuation is just what you think it is, you know?
  • 2:50 - 2:53
    If you went and you were valuing two companies against
  • 2:53 - 2:55
    one another, you'd want to know which was more valuable.
  • 2:55 - 2:59
    Cultures discovered the key feature of valuation thousands of years ago.
  • 2:59 - 3:02
    If you want to compare oranges to windshields, what do you do?
  • 3:02 - 3:04
    Well, you can't compare oranges to windshields.
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    They're immiscible. They don't mix with one another.
  • 3:07 - 3:09
    So instead, you convert them to a common currency scale,
  • 3:09 - 3:12
    put them on that scale, and value them accordingly.
  • 3:12 - 3:15
    Well, your brain has to do something just like that as well,
  • 3:15 - 3:18
    and we're now beginning to understand and identify
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    brain systems involved in valuation,
  • 3:20 - 3:22
    and one of them includes a neurotransmitter system
  • 3:22 - 3:25
    whose cells are located in your brainstem
  • 3:25 - 3:28
    and deliver the chemical dopamine to the rest of your brain.
  • 3:28 - 3:31
    I won't go through the details of it, but that's an important
  • 3:31 - 3:33
    discovery, and we know a good bit about that now,
  • 3:33 - 3:35
    and it's just a small piece of it, but it's important because
  • 3:35 - 3:38
    those are the neurons that you would lose if you had Parkinson's disease,
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    and they're also the neurons that are hijacked by literally
  • 3:40 - 3:42
    every drug of abuse, and that makes sense.
  • 3:42 - 3:45
    Drugs of abuse would come in, and they would change
  • 3:45 - 3:47
    the way you value the world. They change the way
  • 3:47 - 3:50
    you value the symbols associated with your drug of choice,
  • 3:50 - 3:52
    and they make you value that over everything else.
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    Here's the key feature though. These neurons are also
  • 3:55 - 3:59
    involved in the way you can assign value to literally abstract ideas,
  • 3:59 - 4:01
    and I put some symbols up here that we assign value to
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    for various reasons.
  • 4:04 - 4:06
    We have a behavioral superpower in our brain,
  • 4:06 - 4:08
    and it at least in part involves dopamine.
  • 4:08 - 4:12
    We can deny every instinct we have for survival for an idea,
  • 4:12 - 4:16
    for a mere idea. No other species can do that.
  • 4:16 - 4:20
    In 1997, the cult Heaven's Gate committed mass suicide
  • 4:20 - 4:22
    predicated on the idea that there was a spaceship
  • 4:22 - 4:26
    hiding in the tail of the then-visible comet Hale-Bopp
  • 4:26 - 4:30
    waiting to take them to the next level. It was an incredibly tragic event.
  • 4:30 - 4:34
    More than two thirds of them had college degrees.
  • 4:34 - 4:37
    But the point here is they were able to deny their instincts for survival
  • 4:37 - 4:40
    using exactly the same systems that were put there
  • 4:40 - 4:44
    to make them survive. That's a lot of control, okay?
  • 4:44 - 4:46
    One thing that I've left out of this narrative
  • 4:46 - 4:49
    is the obvious thing, which is the focus of the rest of my
  • 4:49 - 4:51
    little talk, and that is other people.
  • 4:51 - 4:54
    These same valuation systems are redeployed
  • 4:54 - 4:56
    when we're valuing interactions with other people.
  • 4:56 - 4:59
    So this same dopamine system that gets addicted to drugs,
  • 4:59 - 5:02
    that makes you freeze when you get Parkinson's disease,
  • 5:02 - 5:05
    that contributes to various forms of psychosis,
  • 5:05 - 5:09
    is also redeployed to value interactions with other people
  • 5:09 - 5:12
    and to assign value to gestures that you do
  • 5:12 - 5:14
    when you're interacting with somebody else.
  • 5:14 - 5:17
    Let me give you an example of this.
  • 5:17 - 5:20
    You bring to the table such enormous processing power
  • 5:20 - 5:23
    in this domain that you hardly even notice it.
  • 5:23 - 5:24
    Let me just give you a few examples. So here's a baby.
  • 5:24 - 5:28
    She's three months old. She still poops in her diapers and she can't do calculus.
  • 5:28 - 5:31
    She's related to me. Somebody will be very glad that she's up here on the screen.
  • 5:31 - 5:34
    You can cover up one of her eyes, and you can still read
  • 5:34 - 5:36
    something in the other eye, and I see sort of curiosity
  • 5:36 - 5:40
    in one eye, I see maybe a little bit of surprise in the other.
  • 5:40 - 5:43
    Here's a couple. They're sharing a moment together,
  • 5:43 - 5:44
    and we've even done an experiment where you can cut out
  • 5:44 - 5:47
    different pieces of this frame and you can still see
  • 5:47 - 5:50
    that they're sharing it. They're sharing it sort of in parallel.
  • 5:50 - 5:52
    Now, the elements of the scene also communicate this
  • 5:52 - 5:55
    to us, but you can read it straight off their faces,
  • 5:55 - 5:58
    and if you compare their faces to normal faces, it would be a very subtle cue.
  • 5:58 - 6:01
    Here's another couple. He's projecting out at us,
  • 6:01 - 6:04
    and she's clearly projecting, you know,
  • 6:04 - 6:07
    love and admiration at him.
  • 6:07 - 6:10
    Here's another couple. (Laughter)
  • 6:10 - 6:15
    And I'm thinking I'm not seeing love and admiration on the left. (Laughter)
  • 6:15 - 6:18
    In fact, I know this is his sister, and you can just see
  • 6:18 - 6:20
    him saying, "Okay, we're doing this for the camera,
  • 6:20 - 6:26
    and then afterwards you steal my candy and you punch me in the face." (Laughter)
  • 6:26 - 6:28
    He'll kill me for showing that.
  • 6:28 - 6:31
    All right, so what does this mean?
  • 6:31 - 6:34
    It means we bring an enormous amount of processing power to the problem.
  • 6:34 - 6:38
    It engages deep systems in our brain, in dopaminergic
  • 6:38 - 6:41
    systems that are there to make you chase sex, food and salt.
  • 6:41 - 6:44
    They keep you alive. It gives them the pie, it gives
  • 6:44 - 6:47
    that kind of a behavioral punch which we've called a superpower.
  • 6:47 - 6:50
    So how can we take that and arrange a kind of staged
  • 6:50 - 6:53
    social interaction and turn that into a scientific probe?
  • 6:53 - 6:56
    And the short answer is games.
  • 6:56 - 7:00
    Economic games. So what we do is we go into two areas.
  • 7:00 - 7:03
    One area is called experimental economics. The other area is called behavioral economics.
  • 7:03 - 7:08
    And we steal their games. And we contrive them to our own purposes.
  • 7:08 - 7:11
    So this shows you one particular game called an ultimatum game.
  • 7:11 - 7:12
    Red person is given a hundred dollars and can offer
  • 7:12 - 7:16
    a split to blue. Let's say red wants to keep 70,
  • 7:16 - 7:20
    and offers blue 30. So he offers a 70-30 split with blue.
  • 7:20 - 7:23
    Control passes to blue, and blue says, "I accept it,"
  • 7:23 - 7:25
    in which case he'd get the money, or blue says,
  • 7:25 - 7:29
    "I reject it," in which case no one gets anything. Okay?
  • 7:29 - 7:33
    So a rational choice economist would say, well,
  • 7:33 - 7:35
    you should take all non-zero offers.
  • 7:35 - 7:39
    What do people do? People are indifferent at an 80-20 split.
  • 7:39 - 7:42
    At 80-20, it's a coin flip whether you accept that or not.
  • 7:42 - 7:45
    Why is that? You know, because you're pissed off.
  • 7:45 - 7:49
    You're mad. That's an unfair offer, and you know what an unfair offer is.
  • 7:49 - 7:51
    This is the kind of game done by my lab and many around the world.
  • 7:51 - 7:54
    That just gives you an example of the kind of thing that
  • 7:54 - 7:58
    these games probe. The interesting thing is, these games
  • 7:58 - 8:01
    require that you have a lot of cognitive apparatus on line.
  • 8:01 - 8:04
    You have to be able to come to the table with a proper model of another person.
  • 8:04 - 8:07
    You have to be able to remember what you've done.
  • 8:07 - 8:09
    You have to stand up in the moment to do that.
  • 8:09 - 8:12
    Then you have to update your model based on the signals coming back,
  • 8:12 - 8:15
    and you have to do something that is interesting,
  • 8:15 - 8:18
    which is you have to do a kind of depth of thought assay.
  • 8:18 - 8:21
    That is, you have to decide what that other person expects of you.
  • 8:21 - 8:24
    You have to send signals to manage your image in their mind.
  • 8:24 - 8:27
    Like a job interview. You sit across the desk from somebody,
  • 8:27 - 8:28
    they have some prior image of you,
  • 8:28 - 8:31
    you send signals across the desk to move their image
  • 8:31 - 8:35
    of you from one place to a place where you want it to be.
  • 8:35 - 8:38
    We're so good at this we don't really even notice it.
  • 8:38 - 8:42
    These kinds of probes exploit it. Okay?
  • 8:42 - 8:44
    In doing this, what we've discovered is that humans
  • 8:44 - 8:46
    are literal canaries in social exchanges.
  • 8:46 - 8:50
    Canaries used to be used as kind of biosensors in mines.
  • 8:50 - 8:53
    When methane built up, or carbon dioxide built up,
  • 8:53 - 8:57
    or oxygen was diminished, the birds would swoon
  • 8:57 - 9:00
    before people would -- so it acted as an early warning system:
  • 9:00 - 9:03
    Hey, get out of the mine. Things aren't going so well.
  • 9:03 - 9:06
    People come to the table, and even these very blunt,
  • 9:06 - 9:09
    staged social interactions, and they, and there's just
  • 9:09 - 9:12
    numbers going back and forth between the people,
  • 9:12 - 9:14
    and they bring enormous sensitivities to it.
  • 9:14 - 9:16
    So we realized we could exploit this, and in fact,
  • 9:16 - 9:19
    as we've done that, and we've done this now in
  • 9:19 - 9:22
    many thousands of people, I think on the order of
  • 9:22 - 9:24
    five or six thousand. We actually, to make this
  • 9:24 - 9:26
    a biological probe, need bigger numbers than that,
  • 9:26 - 9:30
    remarkably so. But anyway,
  • 9:30 - 9:32
    patterns have emerged, and we've been able to take
  • 9:32 - 9:36
    those patterns, convert them into mathematical models,
  • 9:36 - 9:38
    and use those mathematical models to gain new insights
  • 9:38 - 9:40
    into these exchanges. Okay, so what?
  • 9:40 - 9:44
    Well, the so what is, that's a really nice behavioral measure,
  • 9:44 - 9:47
    the economic games bring to us notions of optimal play.
  • 9:47 - 9:50
    We can compute that during the game.
  • 9:50 - 9:53
    And we can use that to sort of carve up the behavior.
  • 9:53 - 9:57
    Here's the cool thing. Six or seven years ago,
  • 9:57 - 9:59
    we developed a team. It was at the time in Houston, Texas.
  • 9:59 - 10:03
    It's now in Virginia and London. And we built software
  • 10:03 - 10:06
    that'll link functional magnetic resonance imaging devices
  • 10:06 - 10:10
    up over the Internet. I guess we've done up to six machines
  • 10:10 - 10:12
    at a time, but let's just focus on two.
  • 10:12 - 10:15
    So it synchronizes machines anywhere in the world.
  • 10:15 - 10:18
    We synchronize the machines, set them into these
  • 10:18 - 10:20
    staged social interactions, and we eavesdrop on both
  • 10:20 - 10:22
    of the interacting brains. So for the first time,
  • 10:22 - 10:26
    we don't have to look at just averages over single individuals,
  • 10:26 - 10:28
    or have individuals playing computers, or try to make
  • 10:28 - 10:31
    inferences that way. We can study individual dyads.
  • 10:31 - 10:34
    We can study the way that one person interacts with another person,
  • 10:34 - 10:37
    turn the numbers up, and start to gain new insights
  • 10:37 - 10:39
    into the boundaries of normal cognition,
  • 10:39 - 10:42
    but more importantly, we can put people with
  • 10:42 - 10:45
    classically defined mental illnesses, or brain damage,
  • 10:45 - 10:49
    into these social interactions, and use these as probes of that.
  • 10:49 - 10:51
    So we've started this effort. We've made a few hits,
  • 10:51 - 10:53
    a few, I think, embryonic discoveries.
  • 10:53 - 10:56
    We think there's a future to this. But it's our way
  • 10:56 - 10:59
    of going in and redefining, with a new lexicon,
  • 10:59 - 11:03
    a mathematical one actually, as opposed to the standard
  • 11:03 - 11:05
    ways that we think about mental illness,
  • 11:05 - 11:07
    characterizing these diseases, by using the people
  • 11:07 - 11:10
    as birds in the exchanges. That is, we exploit the fact
  • 11:10 - 11:15
    that the healthy partner, playing somebody with major depression,
  • 11:15 - 11:18
    or playing somebody with autism spectrum disorder,
  • 11:18 - 11:21
    or playing somebody with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder,
  • 11:21 - 11:25
    we use that as a kind of biosensor, and then we use
  • 11:25 - 11:27
    computer programs to model that person, and it gives us
  • 11:27 - 11:30
    a kind of assay of this.
  • 11:30 - 11:32
    Early days, and we're just beginning, we're setting up sites
  • 11:32 - 11:35
    around the world. Here are a few of our collaborating sites.
  • 11:35 - 11:38
    The hub, ironically enough,
  • 11:38 - 11:41
    is centered in little Roanoke, Virginia.
  • 11:41 - 11:43
    There's another hub in London, now, and the rest
  • 11:43 - 11:47
    are getting set up. We hope to give the data away
  • 11:47 - 11:51
    at some stage. That's a complicated issue
  • 11:51 - 11:54
    about making it available to the rest of the world.
  • 11:54 - 11:55
    But we're also studying just a small part
  • 11:55 - 11:58
    of what makes us interesting as human beings, and so
  • 11:58 - 12:00
    I would invite other people who are interested in this
  • 12:00 - 12:02
    to ask us for the software, or even for guidance
  • 12:02 - 12:04
    on how to move forward with that.
  • 12:04 - 12:07
    Let me leave you with one thought in closing.
  • 12:07 - 12:09
    The interesting thing about studying cognition
  • 12:09 - 12:12
    has been that we've been limited, in a way.
  • 12:12 - 12:15
    We just haven't had the tools to look at interacting brains
  • 12:15 - 12:17
    simultaneously.
  • 12:17 - 12:19
    The fact is, though, that even when we're alone,
  • 12:19 - 12:23
    we're a profoundly social creature. We're not a solitary mind
  • 12:23 - 12:28
    built out of properties that kept it alive in the world
  • 12:28 - 12:32
    independent of other people. In fact, our minds
  • 12:32 - 12:34
    depend on other people. They depend on other people,
  • 12:34 - 12:36
    and they're expressed in other people,
  • 12:36 - 12:40
    so the notion of who you are, you often don't know
  • 12:40 - 12:42
    who you are until you see yourself in interaction with people
  • 12:42 - 12:45
    that are close to you, people that are enemies of you,
  • 12:45 - 12:47
    people that are agnostic to you.
  • 12:47 - 12:51
    So this is the first sort of step into using that insight
  • 12:51 - 12:54
    into what makes us human beings, turning it into a tool,
  • 12:54 - 12:56
    and trying to gain new insights into mental illness.
  • 12:56 - 12:59
    Thanks for having me. (Applause)
  • 12:59 - 13:02
    (Applause)
Title:
What we're learning from 5,000 brains
Speaker:
Read Montague
Description:

Mice, bugs and hamsters are no longer the only way to study the brain. Functional MRI (fMRI) allows scientists to map brain activity in living, breathing, decision-making human beings. Read Montague gives an overview of how this technology is helping us understand the complicated ways in which we interact with each other.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
13:23

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions