Return to Video

Will our kids be a different species?

  • 0:00 - 0:02
    All right. So, like all good stories,
  • 0:02 - 0:04
    this starts a long, long time ago
  • 0:04 - 0:07
    when there was basically nothing.
  • 0:07 - 0:09
    So here is a complete picture of the universe
  • 0:09 - 0:12
    about 14-odd billion years ago.
  • 0:12 - 0:15
    All energy is concentrated into a single point of energy.
  • 0:15 - 0:17
    For some reason it explodes,
  • 0:17 - 0:19
    and you begin to get these things.
  • 0:19 - 0:22
    So you're now about 14 billion years into this.
  • 0:22 - 0:24
    And these things expand and expand and expand
  • 0:24 - 0:26
    into these giant galaxies,
  • 0:26 - 0:27
    and you get trillions of them.
  • 0:27 - 0:29
    And within these galaxies
  • 0:29 - 0:31
    you get these enormous dust clouds.
  • 0:31 - 0:33
    And I want you to pay particular attention
  • 0:33 - 0:34
    to the three little prongs
  • 0:34 - 0:36
    in the center of this picture.
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    If you take a close-up of those,
  • 0:38 - 0:39
    they look like this.
  • 0:39 - 0:42
    And what you're looking at is columns of dust
  • 0:42 - 0:44
    where there's so much dust --
  • 0:44 - 0:49
    by the way, the scale of this is a trillion vertical miles --
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    and what's happening is there's so much dust,
  • 0:52 - 0:53
    it comes together and it fuses
  • 0:53 - 0:57
    and ignites a thermonuclear reaction.
  • 0:57 - 0:58
    And so what you're watching
  • 0:58 - 1:00
    is the birth of stars.
  • 1:00 - 1:01
    These are stars being born out of here.
  • 1:01 - 1:04
    When enough stars come out,
  • 1:04 - 1:06
    they create a galaxy.
  • 1:06 - 1:09
    This one happens to be a particularly important galaxy,
  • 1:09 - 1:11
    because you are here.
  • 1:11 - 1:12
    (Laughter)
  • 1:12 - 1:14
    And as you take a close-up of this galaxy,
  • 1:14 - 1:16
    you find a relatively normal,
  • 1:16 - 1:19
    not particularly interesting star.
  • 1:19 - 1:23
    By the way, you're now about two-thirds of the way into this story.
  • 1:23 - 1:25
    So this star doesn't even appear
  • 1:25 - 1:28
    until about two-thirds of the way into this story.
  • 1:28 - 1:29
    And then what happens
  • 1:29 - 1:30
    is there's enough dust left over
  • 1:30 - 1:32
    that it doesn't ignite into a star,
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    it becomes a planet.
  • 1:34 - 1:39
    And this is about a little over four billion years ago.
  • 1:39 - 1:40
    And soon thereafter
  • 1:40 - 1:42
    there's enough material left over
  • 1:42 - 1:47
    that you get a primordial soup,
  • 1:47 - 1:49
    and that creates life.
  • 1:49 - 1:53
    And life starts to expand and expand and expand,
  • 1:53 - 1:54
    until it goes kaput.
  • 1:54 - 1:58
    (Laughter)
  • 1:58 - 1:59
    Now the really strange thing
  • 1:59 - 2:02
    is life goes kaput, not once, not twice,
  • 2:02 - 2:04
    but five times.
  • 2:04 - 2:06
    So almost all life on Earth
  • 2:06 - 2:09
    is wiped out about five times.
  • 2:09 - 2:10
    And as you're thinking about that,
  • 2:10 - 2:13
    what happens is you get more and more complexity,
  • 2:13 - 2:14
    more and more stuff
  • 2:14 - 2:18
    to build new things with.
  • 2:18 - 2:20
    And we don't appear
  • 2:20 - 2:25
    until about 99.96 percent of the time into this story,
  • 2:25 - 2:29
    just to put ourselves and our ancestors in perspective.
  • 2:29 - 2:33
    So within that context, there's two theories of the case
  • 2:33 - 2:34
    as to why we're all here.
  • 2:34 - 2:36
    The first theory of the case
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    is that's all she wrote.
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    Under that theory,
  • 2:41 - 2:42
    we are the be-all and end-all
  • 2:42 - 2:44
    of all creation.
  • 2:44 - 2:47
    And the reason for trillions of galaxies,
  • 2:47 - 2:49
    sextillions of planets,
  • 2:49 - 2:54
    is to create something that looks like that
  • 2:54 - 2:57
    and something that looks like that.
  • 2:57 - 2:59
    And that's the purpose of the universe;
  • 2:59 - 3:00
    and then it flat-lines,
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    it doesn't get any better.
  • 3:02 - 3:06
    (Laughter)
  • 3:06 - 3:09
    The only question you might want to ask yourself is,
  • 3:09 - 3:14
    could that be just mildly arrogant?
  • 3:14 - 3:16
    And if it is --
  • 3:16 - 3:21
    and particularly given the fact that we came very close to extinction.
  • 3:21 - 3:25
    There were only about 2,000 of our species left.
  • 3:25 - 3:27
    A few more weeks without rain,
  • 3:27 - 3:30
    we would have never seen any of these.
  • 3:30 - 3:36
    (Laughter)
  • 3:36 - 3:41
    (Applause)
  • 3:41 - 3:44
    So maybe you have to think about a second theory
  • 3:44 - 3:47
    if the first one isn't good enough.
  • 3:47 - 3:49
    Second theory is: Could we upgrade?
  • 3:49 - 3:52
    (Laughter)
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    Well, why would one ask a question like that?
  • 3:55 - 3:57
    Because there have been at least 29 upgrades so far
  • 3:57 - 3:59
    of humanoids.
  • 3:59 - 4:02
    So it turns out that we have upgraded.
  • 4:02 - 4:04
    We've upgraded time and again and again.
  • 4:04 - 4:07
    And it turns out that we keep discovering upgrades.
  • 4:07 - 4:09
    We found this one last year.
  • 4:09 - 4:12
    We found another one last month.
  • 4:12 - 4:14
    And as you're thinking about this,
  • 4:14 - 4:16
    you might also ask the question:
  • 4:16 - 4:19
    So why a single human species?
  • 4:19 - 4:21
    Wouldn't it be really odd
  • 4:21 - 4:25
    if you went to Africa and Asia and Antarctica
  • 4:25 - 4:27
    and found exactly the same bird --
  • 4:27 - 4:31
    particularly given that we co-existed at the same time
  • 4:31 - 4:34
    with at least eight other versions of humanoid
  • 4:34 - 4:37
    at the same time on this planet?
  • 4:37 - 4:38
    So the normal state of affairs
  • 4:38 - 4:41
    is not to have just a Homo sapiens;
  • 4:41 - 4:42
    the normal state of affairs
  • 4:42 - 4:46
    is to have various versions of humans walking around.
  • 4:46 - 4:49
    And if that is the normal state of affairs,
  • 4:49 - 4:51
    then you might ask yourself,
  • 4:51 - 4:53
    all right, so if we want to create something else,
  • 4:53 - 4:56
    how big does a mutation have to be?
  • 4:56 - 4:59
    Well Svante Paabo has the answer.
  • 4:59 - 5:01
    The difference between humans and Neanderthal
  • 5:01 - 5:05
    is 0.004 percent of gene code.
  • 5:05 - 5:06
    That's how big the difference is
  • 5:06 - 5:09
    one species to another.
  • 5:09 - 5:13
    This explains most contemporary political debates.
  • 5:13 - 5:15
    (Laughter)
  • 5:15 - 5:18
    But as you're thinking about this,
  • 5:18 - 5:19
    one of the interesting things
  • 5:19 - 5:23
    is how small these mutations are and where they take place.
  • 5:23 - 5:24
    Difference human/Neanderthal
  • 5:24 - 5:26
    is sperm and testis,
  • 5:26 - 5:27
    smell and skin.
  • 5:27 - 5:29
    And those are the specific genes
  • 5:29 - 5:31
    that differ from one to the other.
  • 5:31 - 5:35
    So very small changes can have a big impact.
  • 5:35 - 5:36
    And as you're thinking about this,
  • 5:36 - 5:39
    we're continuing to mutate.
  • 5:39 - 5:42
    So about 10,000 years ago by the Black Sea,
  • 5:42 - 5:44
    we had one mutation in one gene
  • 5:44 - 5:46
    which led to blue eyes.
  • 5:46 - 5:50
    And this is continuing and continuing and continuing.
  • 5:50 - 5:52
    And as it continues,
  • 5:52 - 5:53
    one of the things that's going to happen this year
  • 5:53 - 5:57
    is we're going to discover the first 10,000 human genomes,
  • 5:57 - 6:00
    because it's gotten cheap enough to do the gene sequencing.
  • 6:00 - 6:01
    And when we find these,
  • 6:01 - 6:04
    we may find differences.
  • 6:04 - 6:07
    And by the way, this is not a debate that we're ready for,
  • 6:07 - 6:10
    because we have really misused the science in this.
  • 6:10 - 6:14
    In the 1920s, we thought there were major differences between people.
  • 6:14 - 6:18
    That was partly based on Francis Galton's work.
  • 6:18 - 6:20
    He was Darwin's cousin.
  • 6:20 - 6:22
    But the U.S., the Carnegie Institute,
  • 6:22 - 6:25
    Stanford, American Neurological Association
  • 6:25 - 6:27
    took this really far.
  • 6:27 - 6:30
    That got exported and was really misused.
  • 6:30 - 6:33
    In fact, it led to some absolutely horrendous
  • 6:33 - 6:35
    treatment of human beings.
  • 6:35 - 6:38
    So since the 1940s, we've been saying there are no differences,
  • 6:38 - 6:39
    we're all identical.
  • 6:39 - 6:42
    We're going to know at year end if that is true.
  • 6:42 - 6:44
    And as we think about that,
  • 6:44 - 6:46
    we're actually beginning to find things
  • 6:46 - 6:49
    like, do you have an ACE gene?
  • 6:49 - 6:51
    Why would that matter?
  • 6:51 - 6:55
    Because nobody's ever climbed an 8,000-meter peak without oxygen
  • 6:55 - 6:58
    that doesn't have an ACE gene.
  • 6:58 - 7:00
    And if you want to get more specific,
  • 7:00 - 7:03
    how about a 577R genotype?
  • 7:03 - 7:07
    Well it turns out that every male Olympic power athelete ever tested
  • 7:07 - 7:11
    carries at least one of these variants.
  • 7:11 - 7:12
    If that is true,
  • 7:12 - 7:14
    it leads to some very complicated questions
  • 7:14 - 7:16
    for the London Olympics.
  • 7:16 - 7:18
    Three options:
  • 7:18 - 7:21
    Do you want the Olympics to be a showcase
  • 7:21 - 7:23
    for really hardworking mutants?
  • 7:23 - 7:25
    (Laughter)
  • 7:25 - 7:28
    Option number two:
  • 7:28 - 7:31
    Why don't we play it like golf or sailing?
  • 7:31 - 7:34
    Because you have one and you don't have one,
  • 7:34 - 7:38
    I'll give you a tenth of a second head start.
  • 7:38 - 7:39
    Version number three:
  • 7:39 - 7:41
    Because this is a naturally occurring gene
  • 7:41 - 7:44
    and you've got it and you didn't pick the right parents,
  • 7:44 - 7:47
    you get the right to upgrade.
  • 7:47 - 7:49
    Three different options.
  • 7:49 - 7:51
    If these differences are the difference
  • 7:51 - 7:54
    between an Olympic medal and a non-Olympic medal.
  • 7:54 - 7:57
    And it turns out that as we discover these things,
  • 7:57 - 8:00
    we human beings really like to change
  • 8:00 - 8:02
    how we look, how we act,
  • 8:02 - 8:04
    what our bodies do.
  • 8:04 - 8:08
    And we had about 10.2 million plastic surgeries in the United States,
  • 8:08 - 8:11
    except that with the technologies that are coming online today,
  • 8:11 - 8:14
    today's corrections, deletions,
  • 8:14 - 8:16
    augmentations and enhancements
  • 8:16 - 8:19
    are going to seem like child's play.
  • 8:19 - 8:23
    You already saw the work by Tony Atala on TED,
  • 8:23 - 8:26
    but this ability to start filling
  • 8:26 - 8:29
    things like inkjet cartridges with cells
  • 8:29 - 8:34
    are allowing us to print skin, organs
  • 8:34 - 8:37
    and a whole series of other body parts.
  • 8:37 - 8:38
    And as these technologies go forward,
  • 8:38 - 8:42
    you keep seeing this, you keep seeing this, you keep seeing things --
  • 8:42 - 8:45
    2000, human genome sequence --
  • 8:45 - 8:49
    and it seems like nothing's happening,
  • 8:49 - 8:52
    until it does.
  • 8:52 - 8:55
    And we may just be in some of these weeks.
  • 8:55 - 8:57
    And as you're thinking about
  • 8:57 - 9:00
    these two guys sequencing a human genome in 2000
  • 9:00 - 9:04
    and the Public Project sequencing the human genome in 2000,
  • 9:04 - 9:07
    then you don't hear a lot,
  • 9:07 - 9:11
    until you hear about an experiment last year in China,
  • 9:11 - 9:15
    where they take skin cells from this mouse,
  • 9:15 - 9:17
    put four chemicals on it,
  • 9:17 - 9:20
    turn those skin cells into stem cells,
  • 9:20 - 9:22
    let the stem cells grow
  • 9:22 - 9:25
    and create a full copy of that mouse.
  • 9:25 - 9:28
    That's a big deal.
  • 9:28 - 9:29
    Because in essence
  • 9:29 - 9:31
    what it means is you can take a cell,
  • 9:31 - 9:34
    which is a pluripotent stem cell,
  • 9:34 - 9:36
    which is like a skier at the top of a mountain,
  • 9:36 - 9:40
    and those two skiers become two pluripotent stem cells,
  • 9:40 - 9:42
    four, eight, 16,
  • 9:42 - 9:44
    and then it gets so crowded
  • 9:44 - 9:45
    after 16 divisions
  • 9:45 - 9:48
    that those cells have to differentiate.
  • 9:48 - 9:49
    So they go down one side of the mountain,
  • 9:49 - 9:51
    they go down another.
  • 9:51 - 9:52
    And as they pick that,
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    these become bone,
  • 9:54 - 9:57
    and then they pick another road and these become platelets,
  • 9:57 - 9:59
    and these become macrophages,
  • 9:59 - 10:01
    and these become T cells.
  • 10:01 - 10:03
    But it's really hard, once you ski down,
  • 10:03 - 10:04
    to get back up.
  • 10:04 - 10:10
    Unless, of course, if you have a ski lift.
  • 10:10 - 10:12
    And what those four chemicals do
  • 10:12 - 10:14
    is they take any cell
  • 10:14 - 10:16
    and take it way back up the mountain
  • 10:16 - 10:18
    so it can become any body part.
  • 10:18 - 10:20
    And as you think of that,
  • 10:20 - 10:22
    what it means is potentially
  • 10:22 - 10:24
    you can rebuild a full copy
  • 10:24 - 10:26
    of any organism
  • 10:26 - 10:28
    out of any one of its cells.
  • 10:28 - 10:31
    That turns out to be a big deal
  • 10:31 - 10:34
    because now you can take, not just mouse cells,
  • 10:34 - 10:36
    but you can human skin cells
  • 10:36 - 10:39
    and turn them into human stem cells.
  • 10:39 - 10:43
    And then what they did in October
  • 10:43 - 10:46
    is they took skin cells, turned them into stem cells
  • 10:46 - 10:50
    and began to turn them into liver cells.
  • 10:50 - 10:51
    So in theory,
  • 10:51 - 10:56
    you could grow any organ from any one of your cells.
  • 10:56 - 10:58
    Here's a second experiment:
  • 10:58 - 11:01
    If you could photocopy your body,
  • 11:01 - 11:04
    maybe you also want to take your mind.
  • 11:04 - 11:05
    And one of the things you saw at TED
  • 11:05 - 11:07
    about a year and a half ago
  • 11:07 - 11:08
    was this guy.
  • 11:08 - 11:11
    And he gave a wonderful technical talk.
  • 11:11 - 11:12
    He's a professor at MIT.
  • 11:12 - 11:14
    But in essence what he said
  • 11:14 - 11:16
    is you can take retroviruses,
  • 11:16 - 11:19
    which get inside brain cells of mice.
  • 11:19 - 11:21
    You can tag them with proteins
  • 11:21 - 11:23
    that light up when you light them.
  • 11:23 - 11:27
    And you can map the exact pathways
  • 11:27 - 11:30
    when a mouse sees, feels, touches,
  • 11:30 - 11:33
    remembers, loves.
  • 11:33 - 11:35
    And then you can take a fiber optic cable
  • 11:35 - 11:39
    and light up some of the same things.
  • 11:39 - 11:41
    And by the way, as you do this,
  • 11:41 - 11:43
    you can image it in two colors,
  • 11:43 - 11:45
    which means you can download this information
  • 11:45 - 11:50
    as binary code directly into a computer.
  • 11:50 - 11:52
    So what's the bottom line on that?
  • 11:52 - 11:55
    Well it's not completely inconceivable
  • 11:55 - 11:59
    that someday you'll be able to download your own memories,
  • 11:59 - 12:01
    maybe into a new body.
  • 12:01 - 12:06
    And maybe you can upload other people's memories as well.
  • 12:06 - 12:09
    And this might have just one or two
  • 12:09 - 12:13
    small ethical, political, moral implications.
  • 12:13 - 12:14
    (Laughter)
  • 12:14 - 12:17
    Just a thought.
  • 12:17 - 12:19
    Here's the kind of questions
  • 12:19 - 12:21
    that are becoming interesting questions
  • 12:21 - 12:23
    for philosophers, for governing people,
  • 12:23 - 12:26
    for economists, for scientists.
  • 12:26 - 12:30
    Because these technologies are moving really quickly.
  • 12:30 - 12:31
    And as you think about it,
  • 12:31 - 12:34
    let me close with an example of the brain.
  • 12:34 - 12:36
    The first place where you would expect
  • 12:36 - 12:39
    to see enormous evolutionary pressure today,
  • 12:39 - 12:41
    both because of the inputs,
  • 12:41 - 12:43
    which are becoming massive,
  • 12:43 - 12:45
    and because of the plasticity of the organ,
  • 12:45 - 12:47
    is the brain.
  • 12:47 - 12:50
    Do we have any evidence that that is happening?
  • 12:50 - 12:55
    Well let's take a look at something like autism incidence per thousand.
  • 12:55 - 12:58
    Here's what it looks like in 2000.
  • 12:58 - 13:00
    Here's what it looks like in 2002,
  • 13:00 - 13:04
    2006, 2008.
  • 13:04 - 13:08
    Here's the increase in less than a decade.
  • 13:08 - 13:13
    And we still don't know why this is happening.
  • 13:13 - 13:15
    What we do know is, potentially,
  • 13:15 - 13:17
    the brain is reacting in
  • 13:17 - 13:20
    a hyperactive, hyper-plastic way,
  • 13:20 - 13:22
    and creating individuals that are like this.
  • 13:22 - 13:25
    And this is only one of the conditions that's out there.
  • 13:25 - 13:29
    You've also got people with who are extraordinarily smart,
  • 13:29 - 13:31
    people who can remember everything they've seen in their lives,
  • 13:31 - 13:33
    people who've got synesthesia,
  • 13:33 - 13:34
    people who've got schizophrenia.
  • 13:34 - 13:36
    You've got all kinds of stuff going on out there,
  • 13:36 - 13:38
    and we still don't understand
  • 13:38 - 13:40
    how and why this is happening.
  • 13:40 - 13:43
    But one question you might want to ask is,
  • 13:43 - 13:45
    are we seeing a rapid evolution of the brain
  • 13:45 - 13:47
    and of how we process data?
  • 13:47 - 13:50
    Because when you think of how much data's coming into our brains,
  • 13:50 - 13:54
    we're trying to take in as much data in a day
  • 13:54 - 13:56
    as people used to take in in a lifetime.
  • 13:56 - 13:59
    And as you're thinking about this,
  • 13:59 - 14:01
    there's four theories as to why this might be going on,
  • 14:01 - 14:02
    plus a whole series of others.
  • 14:02 - 14:04
    I don't have a good answer.
  • 14:04 - 14:08
    There really needs to be more research on this.
  • 14:08 - 14:10
    One option is the fast food fetish.
  • 14:10 - 14:12
    There's beginning to be some evidence
  • 14:12 - 14:15
    that obesity and diet
  • 14:15 - 14:16
    have something to do
  • 14:16 - 14:18
    with gene modifications,
  • 14:18 - 14:20
    which may or may not have an impact
  • 14:20 - 14:24
    on how the brain of an infant works.
  • 14:24 - 14:28
    A second option is the sexy geek option.
  • 14:28 - 14:32
    These conditions are highly rare.
  • 14:32 - 14:35
    (Laughter)
  • 14:35 - 14:40
    (Applause)
  • 14:40 - 14:42
    But what's beginning to happen
  • 14:42 - 14:45
    is because these geeks are all getting together,
  • 14:45 - 14:47
    because they are highly qualified for computer programming
  • 14:47 - 14:50
    and it is highly remunerated,
  • 14:50 - 14:53
    as well as other very detail-oriented tasks,
  • 14:53 - 14:55
    that they are concentrating geographically
  • 14:55 - 14:58
    and finding like-minded mates.
  • 14:58 - 15:02
    So this is the assortative mating hypothesis
  • 15:02 - 15:05
    of these genes reinforcing one another
  • 15:05 - 15:07
    in these structures.
  • 15:07 - 15:10
    The third, is this too much information?
  • 15:10 - 15:11
    We're trying to process so much stuff
  • 15:11 - 15:14
    that some people get synesthetic
  • 15:14 - 15:16
    and just have huge pipes that remember everything.
  • 15:16 - 15:19
    Other people get hyper-sensitive to the amount of information.
  • 15:19 - 15:23
    Other people react with various psychological conditions
  • 15:23 - 15:24
    or reactions to this information.
  • 15:24 - 15:27
    Or maybe it's chemicals.
  • 15:27 - 15:29
    But when you see an increase
  • 15:29 - 15:31
    of that order of magnitude in a condition,
  • 15:31 - 15:33
    either you're not measuring it right
  • 15:33 - 15:35
    or there's something going on very quickly,
  • 15:35 - 15:39
    and it may be evolution in real time.
  • 15:39 - 15:42
    Here's the bottom line.
  • 15:42 - 15:44
    What I think we are doing
  • 15:44 - 15:46
    is we're transitioning as a species.
  • 15:46 - 15:51
    And I didn't think this when Steve Gullans and I started writing together.
  • 15:51 - 15:54
    I think we're transitioning into Homo evolutis
  • 15:54 - 15:55
    that, for better or worse,
  • 15:55 - 15:59
    is not just a hominid that's conscious of his or her environment,
  • 15:59 - 16:03
    it's a hominid that's beginning to directly and deliberately
  • 16:03 - 16:06
    control the evolution of its own species,
  • 16:06 - 16:10
    of bacteria, of plants, of animals.
  • 16:10 - 16:12
    And I think that's such an order of magnitude change
  • 16:12 - 16:15
    that your grandkids or your great-grandkids
  • 16:15 - 16:19
    may be a species very different from you.
  • 16:19 - 16:20
    Thank you very much.
  • 16:20 - 16:25
    (Applause)
Title:
Will our kids be a different species?
Speaker:
Juan Enriquez
Description:

Throughout human evolution, multiple versions of humans co-existed. Could we be mid-upgrade now? At TEDxSummit, Juan Enriquez sweeps across time and space to bring us to the present moment -- and shows how technology is revealing evidence that suggests rapid evolution may be under way.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
16:48
Jenny Zurawell edited English subtitles for Will our kids be a different species?
Jenny Zurawell approved English subtitles for Will our kids be a different species?
Jenny Zurawell edited English subtitles for Will our kids be a different species?
Morton Bast accepted English subtitles for Will our kids be a different species?
Morton Bast edited English subtitles for Will our kids be a different species?
Timothy Covell edited English subtitles for Will our kids be a different species?
Timothy Covell added a translation

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions